Man, Technology and Risk A Study on Intuitive Risk Assessment and Attitudes towards Nuclear Energy by O. Renn This translation was undertaken by the Tec:hnik-Sprachendienst G. Beyer GmbH Aachener StraBe 340 D-5000 Koln 41 and they are responsible for the accuracy af the text - I - Abstract Using the instruments of empirical social sciences, a crass-section study was conducted comprising experLments on qualitative risk characteristics, in-depth interviews on mechanisms of risk perception and representative surveys of the public on technical risk sources, in par- ticular with regard to nuclear energy. The results of these studies show that person-related expectations in respect of risk consequences, the possibility of personal influencing control, the severeness of risk consequences and one's own risk propensity playa significant role in the evaluation of risks. - II - Introduction Risk - a multifarious concept: A gambler hoping to win, an actuary computing 11fe expectations, an industrialist estimating his marketing opportunities, a family evaluat- ing the future usefulness of a consumer product, a pa- tient visualizing the success or failure of surgery, a technician investigating the probabilities of skiing accidents or malfunctions - all of them start out from a eommOn concept : risk. But do they always mean the same thing? What is risk? The scientist has a clear-cut and fast answer ,. to this question: Risk is the sum total of all probabilities, P, multiplied by the valuations of possible consequen- ces,c. It 1s on the basis of this formula that risk anal- yses are computed for technological systems, nuclear pow- er stations, road traffic, dams and bridges, but also for the side effects of phar.maceutlcals, for environmen- tal pollution or for noxious substances. But can we use such a formula when it involves personal risk considerations when buying consumer goods, problems of the individual or collective acceptance of technologi- cal risk sources, usefulness or absurdity of leisure oc- cupation, or the psyche of compulsive gamblers? Certain- ly notl But to this date psychology, the science respon- sible for this field, has rather neglected the topic of risk. Many works on decision-making theory, as stated by the social psychologists Janis and Mann, do not even mention the risk concept. This has probably been due to the fact that this word is used to define so many different ideas that its definition has become vague. - III - The present study represents an attempt to bridge this gap in psychological research and to furnish a con- tribution toward a psychology of risk. Such an under- taking of course requires the selection of an exemplary risk source, unless the investigator accepts being bogged down in a multitude of examples and their peculiarities. Nuclear energy was selected as this exemplary risk source. Why? There is hardly any technological facility which has ever stirred the consciences of the western World as nuclear power stations and their associated plants. Hardly any facility has become so associated with risk, and hardly any technology has so shaped the perception of our industrial culture. The selection of this example is less a tribute to the topicality of this conflict than a calculus in the researcher's ter.ms of reference, using the past discussion on the nuclear energy risk and its rational and emotional assimilation by the populace as a data base for an empirical study. For in this exam- ple, attitudes already exist, and object perception struc- tures are well developed. These advantages of object selection can be gained only by accepting an important disadvantage. In a phase of po- litical confrontation between opponents and supporters, studies in the social sciences often have only a rational- izing character. They are used either to substantiate one's own judgement of this issue via a social science analysis, or to provide one's reference group with data and arguments for better armament 1n its conflict with the opponent. Neither was our intention. In order to avoid the danger of allowing our own attitudes to become part of the interpretation, this study includes only analyses on perception processes and attitude structures of the survey respondents, without any attempt at evaluation or criticism of the responses. Of course, this approach does not preclude that concepts such as risk, risk estimation and risk perception are subjected to conceptual clarifica- tion. - IV In spite of the exemplary approach the discussion 1s not confined to the nuclear energy example. The theoretical analysis and its empirical translation include other risk sources from pharmaceuticals to skiing as comparison cri- teria, 1n order to cover higher-level patterns of risk perception and risk assimilation. Comparative analysis of different ~lsk sources broadens the spectrum of the perception fields analyzed and allows the discussion of results covering several objects. This book describes, 1n concise and easily understandable form,the most impor- tant results and conclusions of more than two years of research in this fleld . For those readers particular- ly interested 1n this subject, the final report on this research project has been published in six volumes in the form of JU1-Reports by the JUlich Nuclear Research Centre. Most of the Figures have been adopted from this comprehensive version. The book is organized into five major chapters: Part I presents a critical discussion and summary of the exist- ing literature on risk perception problems, intuitive technology evaluation and nuclear energy attitudes. Part II provides a review of our own theoretical approach and the basic concept used for the empirical analysis. In spite of all our attempts to achieve easy understand- ing in terms of phraseology and terminology, the two chapters are characterized by social science concepts and, therefore, may be somewhat difficult to follow for readers not experienced in this field of research. There- fore, all the important methodological stipulations are repeated once again in the presentation of the empirical resul ts in Parts III - V, so that readers who are inter- ested only in the results can pass over the theoretical chapters . I ask the reader's indulgence for the result- ing, unavoidable redundance in the presentation. - v - The third chapter is concerned with two social psychology experiments intended to contribute toward clarification of the intuitive evaluation of risky situations. Part IV serves the same purpose. It describes and interprets the results of two comprehensive surveys on the evaluation of different risk sources and their determinant factors. The last part 1s exclusively concerned with nuclear energy perception and an analysis of attitude components with respect to nuclear energy facilities and their deter.mlnlnq quantities. The results presented in this Part are based on a representative survey conducted in five towns in the State of North Rhine-Westphalia, a state within the Federal Republic of Germany. Finally, the book includes a brief synopsis of the entire study and an index listing and defining the most important concepts . Publication of this book would have been impossible without the cooperation of Prof. Dr. G. Wiswede, Prof. Or. E. K. Scheuch and Dr. E. MUnch. I am obliged to Professors Wiswede and Scheuch, who supported part of this study as a doctoral thesis, for their critical reviews and many suggestions and improvement proposals. My special appreci- ation is owed to Dr. E. MUnch for his support in the realization of the research project described in this book and for his vigorous support of my work. I am equally obliged to the late Mr. Beyer for the excellent job of translating the German original version into English. Finally, my special gratitude is extended to Mrs. Tanz, who at considerable effort produced this book from my handwritten manuscript. JUlich, January 1981 Dr. O. Renn Table of Contents Abstract Introduction Table of Contents List of Figures Foreword Swmnary Part I - VI - Page I II VI IX XII XV Risk Perception and Acceptance : 1 A Bibliographical Review Systematic Summary of Different Research concepts 2 Decision Analysis and Risk Theory Concepts 6 Normative Decision-Making 6 Descriptive Decision-Making 9 conclusions on Risk Theory and DecisionAnalysis 19 Economic Concepts 20 Individual Psychology Concepts 22 Social Psychology Concepts 26 Sociological Concepts 41 Open Questions and PhenomenaRequiri"ng Explanation 60 Part II Risk and Risk Perception - Basic Theoretical Concept Defini tions Risk Concept Risk Perception Concept Risk Acceptance Concept Probabilistic Risk Analysis for Technologies The Graduated Rationality Model 63 P,4 64 67 70 80 88 Atti tudes and Acceptance 94 Description of Different Research Projects for 102 Empirical Analysis Survey Instruments and Study Cases 104 No. - VII - Part III Socio-Psychological Experiments on Specific Risk Perception Problems - The Medical Capsule Experiment - Experimental Setup - Results of the Medical Capsule Experiment - The Poisoned Water Experiment - Experimental Setup - Results of the Poisoned Water Experiment Part IV Individual Risk Perception - Result of Comprehensive Studies - Effect of Risk Levels (Expected Values) on the Evaluation of Risk Sources - Homogeneity of Risk-Benefit Estimates Made by Different Population Groups 109 110 113 116 119 125 126 142 - Effect of Ideas on the Characteristics of the 149 Individual Risk Sources (Belief System) - Effect of Qualitative Risk Characteristics 167 - Effect of Risk Propensity and Benefit Orientation 185 - Effect of Demographic and Social Characteristics 190 - Overall Analysis of the Factors Influencing the 199 Perception and Acceptance of Risks Part V Nuclear Energy Attitudes and their Determinants - 217 Result of Sampling Surveys - Post-War Nuclear Energy Attitudes of the Population 218 of the Federal Republic of Germany - Effect of the Three Mile Island Accident on the ~8 population - Nuclear Energy Compared to Other Energy Sources 234 - Attitudes Toward Nuclear Energy and their Internal 249 Structure - Significance of Personal Value Preferences and ~4 Socio-Political Beliefs for the Formation of Nuclear Energy Attitudes - Effect of Social and Demographic Characteristics on Nuclear Energy Attitudes - Nuclear Energy and Sex - Nuclear Energy Attitudes and Age 275 276 278 - VIII - - Nuclear Energy Attitudes and Stratification - Nuclear Energy Attitudes and Political Party Preference - Nuclear Energy Attitudes and Other Social Categories 278 289 - Confidence 1n Institutions and Other Reference 289 Groups - Attitude of the Population to Specific Questions ~7 of Nuclear Energy Use - Economic Growth and Energy Consumption - Attitude Toward Citizens' Initiatives 297 299 - Perception of the Waste Disposal Problem 301 - Summary of all Factors Influencing Nuclear Energy 304 Attitudes - General Conclusion - 1. Information Reception - 2. Development of Beliefs - 3. Attribution of Probabilities - 4. Evaluation of Beliefs - 5. Development of a Consistent Attitude - 6. Acceptance Decision - 7. Rationalization - 8. Risk Perception and Social Research JO~ 311 312 313 314 317 318 318 319 Index of Technical Terminology with Definitions 32~ References 335 - IX - List of Figures Page No. Fig. 1: Risk Acceptance Model in a Two-Factor Psychological Space (cf . R6glin) Fig. 2: Risk Acceptance as a Function of Risk Characteristics (cf. Fischhoff et al.) Fig. 3: The Perception of Various Risk Dimensions (Risk Evaluation of Nuclear Waste) Fig . 4: Attitude Dimensions According to Fishbein Model (IAEA Risk Assessment Group) Fig. 5: Types of Risk-Benefit Distribution Fig. 6: Outcome-Exposure Pathways Fig. 7: Methods to Determine Optimum Risk Minimization Fig . 8: The Model of Graduated Risk Perception Fig . 9: Model of the Risk Acceptance Weighing Process Fig. 10: Basic Attitude Concept Fig. 11: Results of the Medical Capsule Experiment 27 33 35 39 73 83 85 91 95 99 111 Fig . 12: Discrepancy Between True and Perceived Fatalities 129 (U.S. Data acc. to Fischhoff) Fig . 13a: Discrepancy Between True and Pereeived Fatalities for an Average Year (German Data, Logarithmic Scale) 133 Fig. 13b: Discrepancy Between True and Perceived Fatalities for a Catastrophic Year (German Data, Logarithmic 115 Scale) Fig. 13c: Discrepancy Between True and Perceived Fatalities for an Average Year (German Data, Mixed Scale) 137 Fig. 14: Mean ValUes of Three Independently Measured Risk-Benefit Estimations Fig. 15: The Estimation of Benefit and Risk - A Comparison Between German and American Data Fig. 16. Factor-Score Patters of the Belief Scale: Percentage of Declared Variance 143 153 - x - Fig. 17: Aggregate Belief Structure Factors (Summation of Factorial Loads) a) Alcohol, Smoking, Heroin 157 b) X-Rays, Electrical Appliances, Skiing 159 0) Automation, Motorcars, Air Traffic 161 d) Pesticides, Nuclear Energy, Coal 163 Fig. 18: Factor-Score Patterns of the Qualitative Risk Characteristics: Multiple Regression Coefficients FiS!. 19 I Comparison of German and American Values for Qualitative Risk Characteristics FiS!. 20: The Qualitative Risk Characteristics as a Function of Risk-Benefit Estimation and the Risk Evaluation FiS!. 21a: Aggregate Qualitative Risk Factors (Voluntariness Versus Equal Risk-Benefit Distribution) FiS!. 21b: Aggregate Qualitative Risk Factors (Voluntariness Versus Severeness of Consequences) FiS!.21c: Aggregate Qualitative Risk Factors (Equal Risk- Benefit Distribution Versus Severeness of Consequences) FiS!. 22: Weighting of Belief Factors as a Function of Class Membership (Pretest Results) FiS!. 23: Weighting of Aggregate Belief Factors as a Function of Class Membership (Main Study) FiS!s.24a to 24e: Graphic Representation of the Multlple Regression Values of all Independent Variables for the" Semantic Differential of Nine Risk Sources Flga 25: Estimation of Nuclear Energy as a Personal 169 173 175 177 179 181 195 197 201 al3 205" 207 209 Preference for the Future, as a Function of the 239 Belief System and the Semantic Differential for Nuclear Energy FiS!. 26: Discrepancy Between Desired and Expected Future of Nuclear Energy 241 - XI - Fig . 27: Attributive Profiles for Coal, Solar and Nuclear Energy (Semantic Differentials) Fig. 28: Relationship Between Risk-Benefit Estimation and Behavioural Intention Fig. 29: Path Analysis Model of Nuclear Energy Attitudes and Positions in a Referendum Fill· 30: Specific Pattern of General and Personal Values in a TWo-Factor Psychological Space Fill· 31: Effect of Socio-Political Attitudes in the Risk-Benefit Estimation of Nuclear Energy Fill· 32 : Attitude-Forming Variables as a Function of Stratum and Position 247 259 261 267 273 231 - XII - Foreword This book is primarily concerned with the perception of technological risk and, in particular, with attitudes to- ward nuclear energy. In the case of this controversial topic there 1s the danger of sciencQ being forced into a mere- ly palliative role of post-factum justification and ra- tionalization of positions already taken. Therefore, I should like to assure the reader from the outset that the author of this book has exhibited a well-balanced and aloof attitude, and has succeeded well in avoiding this trap and communicating a great variety of differentiated information. The following problem areas are involved: (1) risk perception and intuitive risk assessment 1 (2) development and structure of attitudes toward nuclear energy; (3) psychic and social influencing factors which are the causes of these attitudes. The author initially discusses perception and acceptance for different risk sources on a quite general basis. Risk acceptance 1s defined as a cognitive process of weighing between subjectively weighted benefits and de- trimental consequences. Previous studies on risk accept- ance are critically discussed and found to be excessively limited . The author's own approach leads to a phased mod- el consisting of the following steps : Collection of rel- evant cognitions on risks, attribution of subjective pro- babilities to these cognitions, weighting of these proba- bilities and comparison of these weighted cognitions. - XIII - In order to determine the ranking of (naive) perception and acceptance of risks the author proposes an additional comparison to a type of objective risk measures, using the expert evaluations as a basis. But such a comparison is highly problematic since everyone knows how often the experts have been wrong in this branch of the natural sciences and how often they hold different views. More- over, everybody knows that many imponderables enter into the quasi-objective risk evaluation of an expert. Apart from the problematic basic questions (Who is an expert? To what degree is he an expert? To which group does he belong?), and apart from the risk evaluations which are difficult to grasp and often can be weighted only arbi- trarily, the problem in the final analysis involves a finite selection of possibilities and occurrences which can be included in the calculation and, finally, finite propagation paths with respect to detrimental consequen- ces: it is impossible to calculate probabilities of the occurrence of events of which nobody has ever thought before. The author is quite aware of these reservations with re- spect to "objectivity", but would not forgo this field of reference of the quasi-objective evaluation of the ex- perts, because otherwise the naive perceptions of laymen and the calculations of experts would be compared on a level of equality. Rather, Renn attempts to develop the gradual differences in reality content by means of a mo- del of graduated rationality in the perception of risks, which can certainly be considered a pace-setting achieve- ment. The empirical analysis itself is concerned with percep- tion processes of risk sources and with the development and structure of attitudes, specifically with respect to nuclear energy, as well as the internal and external fac- tors which influence these attitudes. The implementation of the empirical study - ranging from its operationaliza- tion, which was sometimes extremely difficult and complex, all the way to the well-balanced interpretation of the - XIV - results - reveals mastery of the methodological instru- ments of the social sciences. ~Some of the results con- firm previous studies, while others go far beyond the present state of the art and reveal a great variety of interesting data. For instance, one important informa- tion item 1s that nuclear energy opinions are highly af- fected by emotions but are still ambivalent to the large majority. This result also appears to reflect interna- tional aspects since comparable data from the United States and Japan also demonstate this ambivalence and rigidity of the opinion body. In contrast to the pic- ture broadcast by the media it 1s also characteristic that most of the nuclear energy opponents feel isolated and that - in a resigned attitude? - they do not believe in the necessity of nuclear energy but are nevertheless convinced that it will come about in the Federal Republic of Germany, regardless of whether the citizens want it or not . Insofar it appears that these results represent an important correction to the widespread opinion that the opposition to nuclear energy is straight-lined and un- compromising. The author has presented an empirical study which ranks at the very forefront of the presently topical scientific discussion and is probably the best-founded study of this subject at present. On the other hand, the author in his mastery of the sophisticated methods of empirical social research and, at the same time, of socia-psycho- logical and sociological phenomena, proves to be a good advocate of interdisciplinary, problem-oriented research. Cologne, January 1981 Prof. Dr. G. Wlswede Cologne University - xv - Sununary Previous studies on risk perception by the population have been based predominantly on three methodological concepts: - the descriptive decision analysis model - themodel of risk-specific perception influence factors such as voluntariness or personal control, and - the attitude model. The main emphasis of studies conducted to date has been in the field of perception-specific risk approaches which, on the one hand, were developed quasi indirectly 1n the "Revealed Risk Analysis" from the history of the acceptance of risk sources (approach by Starr) or, on the other hand, measured by direct surveys with a view to their significance (approach by Fischhoff et al.). Studies on the attitude concept, which is largely pursued by the IAEA Risk Assessment Group 1n Vienna, relate more strongly to the cognitive structure of risk perception. Finally, decision analysis studies are presently being conducted at the University of Southern California (von Winterfeldt,Edwards) , dealing with the value conceptions of relevant groups in the nuclear controversy, where the perception of risk aspects is being understood as an attribution process of prior value com- mitments to concrete risk objects. On the basis of these studies, our aim was to develop an integrative approach to the measurement of perception patterns for a variety of risk sources. The relevant theoretical starting pOint was a mental risk decision model intended to observe and empirically COver the process ranging from information uptake to the behaviour intention in respect of the object in question. The individual stations of risk perception were characterized as follows: - information uptake, - the forming of general beliefs, -~- _ the attribution of probabilities according to the degree of one's own involvement and typical patterns of generalization, _ the evaluation of beliefs by means of attributive biases, qualitative risk characteristics and dispositive as well as sltuatlve factors, - the internal coordination of beliefs (consistence finding) - the acceptance decision, - the forming of an attitude and of behavioural intentions resulting therefrom, _ the immunization of the attitude against inward and outward doubts (rationalizing). For the empirical transformation of this concept, two different comprehensive interviews to elucidate belief patterns about various risks and their weights, two 80c10- psychological experiments on the significance of qualitative risk characteristics and one representative survey on attitude formation and its determinants were conducted in the Federal Republic of Germany. Part of the questions were selected. so as to per.m1t direct comparisons with parallel American and French studies. The results of this very extensive study, comprising twa years of research, may be summarized as follows: - Most of those interviewed possess relatively homogeneous evaluation criteria for an intuitive risk assessment and arrive at similar results in the evaluation of risk-benefit relations for different risk sources. The classification of risk sources according to the level of their overall risk practically does not reveal any regional, temporary of stratum-specific differences. Even a comparison between the populations in the United States and the Federal Republic of Germany results in similar patterns of risk estimation. - XVII - - Although most of those interviewed can reflect relatively well the expected values of losses (fatalities, injured persons, damage to property per year), these estimations are taken as evaluation criteria for the risk-benefit estimation to a minor extent only. The evaluation of risks is governed essentially by the following factors: - perceived consequences of a risk source for oneself or close associates, - perceived consequences relative generally to health, safety and future scope of freedom, - personal posslbl11ts of influencing the risk, - the possibility of far-reaching and fatal losses, - the personal readiness to take risks. The first two aspects were defined as cognitive, the following two aspects as qualitative risk characteristics, and the fifth as dispositive risk propensity. With the aid of these five variables, between 44 and 72% of the variance of the risk-benefit estimation could be explained, depending on the risk source. - Demographic and social characteristics play a subordinate role in the evaluation of risks. Only the sex of those interviewed has an influence on perception, however not directly, but via the devious path of 'special sensitivity to risk consequences relative to health and genetics" and "personal risk propensity". Cognate attitude patterns and general value orientation are of significance only 1f the associations with the risk source are concerned primarily with risk consequences for the economic,society and future life style. This 1s the case, for instance, in connection with nuclear energy and plant-protective agents, but not in connection with coal energy, computers (despite the problems of data protection) or x-ray equipment. - XVIII - _ The nuclear energy risk has a particularly ambivalent perception structure due to attributive biases, on account of the negative, perceived qualitative risk characteristics (non-voluntary, high hazard potential) and because of the fear of long-term damage possibilities which cannot be recognized today. Although the majority of those interviewed was convinced of the fact that nuclear energy 1s an economic necessity and will play a significant role for the future supply of energy, they argue, however, that it involves completely new risk dimensions and cannot be calculated in a scientific analysis. - This ambivalence has left the basic proponents of nuclear power with a dissonant attitude structure, i.e. the beliefs of those interviewed are very inhomogeneous and, in part, contradictory. Their behaviour intentions are accordingly limited, and their doubts about themselves are particularly pronounced. - Determined opponents of nuclear energy, on the other hand, are neither convinced of the economic necessity nor of the harmlessness of the risk. They possess a very homo- geneous attitude structure and, accordingly, a stronger behaviour motivation. They exhibit, however, a pro- nounced resiqnative belief that "it is too late for the nuclear case now· and that, in the long run, they can no longer do anything to prevent the expansion of the nuclear energy sector. This pessimistiC view of the future was defined by us as reality stress . It often has a socially destabilizing effect, expressing itself, for instance, 1n a disintegration from society (withdrawal from society) or, On the other hand, by rigorous activism. - XIX - - The possibilities of a meaningful dialogue between pro- ponents and opponents of nuclear energy are by no means exhausted. Both the opponents and proponents of nuclear energy are still rather inclined to trust in the state- ments of expert scientists as long as these cannot be clearly identified as representing the interests of industry. Moreover, 1n the case of the proponents, a special dependence on pro-nuclear reference groups 1s to be observed which, no doubt, is based on the fact that they, in particular, seek the support of others due to their ambivalent attitude structure. The opponents have less confidence 1n institutional reference groups and feel more strongly attracted to information groups (e.g. local environmentalists). As a recommendation for continuing the dialogue between proponents and opponents of nuclear energy, we consider it necessary that the topics of such conversation should rather be oriented to the perceived consequences of nuclear energy utilization than to the discussion of risk expected values, which are in any case little contested. - 1 - Part I R1sk Perception and Acceptance: A Bibliographical Review - 2 - Systematic Summary of Different Research Concepts The problems involved in the risk acceptance of nuclear facilities in almost all western industrial nations has resulted in an avalanche of psychological and social science literature devoted to the questions of the development, origins and consequences of the present risk acceptance crisis. In spite of this broad spectrum of available scientific analyses most of these studies have been limited to the application or transfer of sub-theories to the present-day situation and attempt, on the basis of a particular theoretical perspective, to describe the nuclear energy conflict and reveal its causes. On the other hand, generally oriented and inter- disciplinary studies on a broad empirical basis are rarely encountered. A brief selection of some important research concepts and theories flicts will on risk acceptance and nuclear be presented in this chapter. energy con- It goes without saying that a selection of this type can never be comprehensive or representative: rather, it 1s the objective of this description to provide a systematic review of the broad spectrum of scientific literature avai~able and comparative explanations of the intentions and results of studies accomplished to date. This re- view does not include publications which also provide a summary of existing concepts but do not represent original theories of their own (cf., for instance, von der Che, 179,288; Paschen, '79,307). [9uotattons are ordered 1n the follOWing manner: author, year of publication, number 1n alphabetic IndexJ In accordance with SCientific conventions and the personal categorization of the authors,the individual studies will be classified into the following scientific fields: - economics-oriented concepts: deCision analysis concepts: - risk theory concepts! - indiVidual psychology concepts; SOCial psychology concepts! - SOCiological and political SCience concepts. - 3 - Table 1 contains a systematic classification of concepts and their most important proponents. This list,ef course,is incom- plete, and the classification is often problematic. However, it appears suitable and sufficiently accurate for an initial orientation on the spectrum of possibilities. Table 1: Systematic Classification of ExplanatopY Concepts SCientific field of classification Economics Decision analysis Theoretical framework Marginal utility theory Economic theory of politics Normative risk assess- ment Normative decision- making theory Descriptive decision- making theory Summary de- scription of the concept Proponents (selection) Determination of marginal cost and util- ity, comparison of individuals, groups and society Felix Renn Demand maximl- Downs, Frey, zatlon via Tltz resource mobilization, pol! tical in- fluence Optimum proce- dures for cost and benefit estimation, risk minimiza- tion and selec- tion of alter- natives Optimization of procedures to guide decision- makers to the most rational decision pos- sible Definl tion of the determi- nants of the factual deci- sion-making process Rowe, Lowrance, Kates, Fischhoff, Sagan Coombs, Okrent, Raiffa,Edwards Janis/Mann, Pollatsek, Tversky, Vlek, Stallen,Kogan, von Winterfeldt Jungermann (contd.) ,ntific field :lassification < theory :hology lal psychology - 4 - Theoretical framework Revealed pre- ference analysis Referred or expressed preference analysis Psycho- analysis Psychological reduction theory Risk per- ception Risk social- ization Attitude concept Communica- tion concept Symbolic re- duction theory Summary de- scription of the concept Past risks as indicators for the evaluation of new risks Empirical deter- mination of risk elements through surveys Transfer of psychoanalytical concepts and ar- chaic types to the underlying causes of nuclear energy attitudes Proponents (selection) Starr, Cohen Fischhoff, Slavic, Lichtenstein WHO Report, Schild, v . Erichsen, WUnschrnann, Tublana Transfer of Pahner, psychic mechanisms Pelicier to the perception of nuclear energy Perception effects in risk assessment (at- tributve biases) Learning of interpretation patterns for risk evaluation Attitude toward the subject re- quires risk acceptance Maynard, Tversky, Fischhoff, Slovlc,Vlek, Kogan, Bler- brauer, Frantzen, Schmid-Jllrg (Battelle) Gutmann (Battelle) Cohen/Hansel Otway,Niehaus Davis, (v . Buiren) Fishbein The conflict is Goerke the result of (Eisenhart, misguided and Crebsbach) distorted communi- catlon The nuclear energy conflict is rep- Rllglin, resentatlve for Dumenil, symbolic societal HofstKtter or psychological issues (contd.) Scientific field of classification Sociology - 5 - Theoretical framework Sociology of the sciences Empirical- functional- istic concept System ana- lysis concept Conflict theory con- cept Summary de- scription of the concept Conflicts are the result and expression of scientific orientation pro- cesses Conflicts are functional/dys- functional for future value orientation Crisis in the interchange relationship between nuclear system and polit- ical periphery Dispute repre- sents conflict of interests and participation Proponents (selection) Nowotny, Tschledel, Nelkln, Weingast LUbbe,Schoeck Douglin, Tognaco!, Battelle, BMFT II Study Gripp Normative- Conflicts lead Scharloth, Paschen, Andritzky Nelkin, Kitscheet Schumacher, Mo6mann, Gladitz democratic to more demo- (participa- cracy tory research) Neo-Marxism Orthodox Marxism Conflicts are Offe, caused by clasp Metz contradictions and capitalistic crises Conflicts are Lob caused by the realization of monopolistic capital exploita- ti"on interests The overall matrix seen from this Table exhibits a very broad spectrum of concepts whose internal logic frequently predetermines the approach and sometimes even the results. It would be beyond the scope of this study to discuss in detail each of these concepts and to subject them to critical - 6 - examination. However, it seems important to sununarlze briefly the basic concepts underlying the existing studies and to highlight the spectrum of questions which have remained unresolved. Decision Analysis and Risk Theory Concepts ~Q~~S!Y~_Q~g!~!Qn=~~~!ng Decision-making and risk theory studies are motivated either by the problem as to an optimum procedure for selecting the best option from a number of alternatives (normative theory), or . by the problem of describing the actual selection process, 1.e. describing how individuals, groups or institutions select an option (descriptive theory). In addition, reference 1s made to a so-called explicative theory which has been used to develop the mathematical and logical framework for rational decision- making and sometimes also to a typological theory which, in a manner of speaking, involves a systematic identifica- tion and classification of all types and possibilities of decision-making. The distinction between normative and descriptive or explicative and tyPological deCision analysis should not be understood as implying strict compart- mentalization. Even the development of proposals for rational decision-making cannot forgo the subjective evaluat.ion of alternatives for action.. Also, the descrip- tion of actual behaviour must be based on certain logical assumptions of behaviour because otherwise the typification or generalization of behaviour patterns would be impossible. Rather, this distinction applies to the purpose of a study: either to give decls1on- making aids or to define existing forms of decision-making. - 7 - But how do these theories relate to the problem as to the ing risk acceptance of nuclear energy? a definition of risk acceptance at Without present- this early point it appears to be intuitively eVident that any con- sideration as to the acceptance of risk represents a decision-making problem. Is the probability of negative effects worth the benefit associated with the source of risk? In order to respond to this question the likely procedure is to collect the ris k aspects, to weight them with the probability of their occurrence and to compare them to the benefit aspects . The result of this balancing process 1s a risk acceptance decision . Let us first consider the proposals presented in the literature to justify normative risk acceptance thresholds, i.e. the determination of a point where risk accep- tance appears justified . The great variety of exist- ing solution models can be reduced to s even basic variants. These include the following: - The arbitrary establishment of a risk acceptance threshold on the basis of plausible considerations (such as the use of production processes optimized for safety engineering), natural standards 'g " < " 12600 PI " 0 ,. " '< • Alkohol PI PI • '" to " " 10800 • ~ 0< ~ He~oln Haushaltsunlalle >< (I) (f) I--' ro~CtI w p., t1 ::s 9 000 -...,J '" .. e. ~ 7200 " - PI it t'Ij 540°1 Elektr. Haushaltsgeriite. " . " o " 3600 .. ~ l~ 0. KKW .Pflanzenschulz hj 1 800 • • Kohlekraftwerk ~ statistlsch _ _ _ .Rontgen e. errechnete Tote 0 ::;: r-0,87 r2-0,77 0 0,5 1 1,5 2 2,5 3 3,5 4 45 5 .. ' ~ log X _ 140- If the different spreads are incorporated and a mixed logarithmic-linear correlation i. used, the relation between the two median classes will rise to a value of .0.81. Comparison between the computed statistical data for a catastrophic year and the corresponding sub- jective estimates reveals a greater difference be- tween the two classes of variables. Apparently, moat ri8ks are given a relatively 8imilar estimate, since ten of the thirteen risk sources are located within a narrow range between values 6 and 8, while the real value8 show a dispersion between 2 and 10 (logarithmic scale). Therefore, in a normal year . it appears that ·common sense- can produce a rela- tively good estimate of 108.es. However, where intuitive yardsticks do not exist, which applies to the claasification of cataatrophic developments, relatively constant 10.8 rates are stated without more ,detailed differentiation between risk type., or extreme orders of magnitude are selected. As a result, the correlation coefficient between real and 'perceived values i. only 0.39 in thi8 easel the relation i8 not 8ignificant (p • 0.10). ~f we atart out from the result that the risk level and ita,J',rob8bil1 ty i,; perceived relatively correctly by the population ~r an average year ,tlte question immediately ariaes whether th~8e perceived parameters also determine th ' ' . e . subjective evaluation of the rial< source. And here a Surprising r~8ult ,is obtained. Almost all correlation analY8eS ~de betwee~ ~ialt estimates and information on future, procedure with respect' -t~ "risk on the one hand and 'all the possibl 1 " , ' , e O. 0.. indices , on the, other hand practically fail to reveal any relationship. Regardless of whether we start out from the perceived number of deaths, injured or property damaged, regardless of whether we construct several indices of these three parameters, whether we divide the risk level by the number of potentially affected persons, whether we use the date for normal or catastrophic years, whether we average the data for normal or catastrophic years under several different procedures (using ratios of 1.50, 1.100 or 1.250) - it is impossible to find a clear-cut relationship to risk-benefit evaluation. The same result has been found in the American studies. Similar to the present study, theBe investigators found a very low agreement between estimAted risk level and risk evaluation. However, the correlation improved significantly when the data for estimAting the catastrophic year were included (Slovic et al, '79, 372, p. 13 ff.). For the German semple even this relationship is not significant. Several studies on the acceptance of medical risks have arrived at the same conclusion. The acceptance problem is only slightly influenced by the estimate of the risk level but predominantly by the evaluation of risk con- 8equences and the motivations for risk acceptance (Pochin, '75, 317, p. 189). All of these studies confirm the result that the perceived risk levels represent only one, possibly low, influencing factor in the estimation of risk sources. At normal averages the risk level estimates of experts end laymen are relatively homogeneous. However, the experts con- sider these estimates to be a normative baais for the acceptance decision (this has been confirmed empirically through a survey of experts conducted by Slovic et aI, .'79, 372, p. 12), while laymen consider it to be only one Weighting factor among many. 142 - Homogeneity of Risk-Benefit Estimates Made by Different Population Groups Prior to discu.sing ·the na~alquestion a. to the causes of the deviation of the intuiUve ri.k-benefit e.timates from the level of the perceived 10 •• rate, a brief ex- planation of the structure of risk-benefit estimates i. of special significance. In all three surveys the inter- view subjects were requested to state a value on a acale from -3 to +3 which in their opinion beat represented the ratio of benefit to detriment for the rlak source in question. This variable ia referred to a. risk- benefit estimate below. In addition, the interview sub- jects were requested in questionnaire II to grade bene- fits and detriments separat.ly on a scal. from 0 to 10. Another index ~r the risk-benefit estimate was obtained fram these two variables by summation. Consequently, the following variables were available for the comparative study of independent and dependent measured values on risk~benefit estimation, _. ·estimate 11 risk-benefit esUmate for 100 interview ' Subjects using questionnaire 1/ estimate 2, risk-benefit estimate for 100 interview subjects using questionnaire 2/ estimate 3. medians of the d1fferencebetween the . sep~rately determined benefit and detri- ~nt,.e.timat" (questionnaire 2) / elt1miot.: 4. ' rtak'-benefit estimats for 507 interview ".ubject. ' Using ·questionnair. 3. The ·distdbut.ion of-th,,"medians for ' .ach of th ... four 8stimat •• ··h • "b" . . . as , aen: plotted in Ficjtir. 15; Good ·agreem.nt between" the'mealilns la°seeneat -first irlahce. .This i. R'.k .. aenem: £.f'maUon Survey 1 Survey 1 V8 Survey 2 Survey 2 va Survey 3 Alcohol Smoklnll Heroin Correlatlone Home Appllanc •• Home Appllanc •• X-Rey Coel X-Rey Motor V.hlcle Alrcre't Alrcr.1t Motor Vehicle Automation at Work Skllnll Automation at Work Pe.tleld •• Nucl.ar Energy Nuclear Energy P •• tlcld •• RI.k·Benetit eatimaUon Survey3 Rlek- Benefit Eetimatlon Survey2 145 _ a striking result, considering that it involved three completely different samples. The same result is also obtained after differentiating by survey locations. With the exception of the nuclear energy variable, there are no significant differences in risk estimation between interview subjects living in JUlich and in Kerpen. This applies equally to the first and the second questionnaire. The dispersion spread of the individual categories is also relatively low. This homogeneity of the response behaviour is retained as a basic structure even in the case of highly different criteria differentiation. Apparently the evaluation pattern. of individuals are similar - at least for the 13 risk sources included in these studies. This fact allows two conclusions. Risk-benefit estimates of different risk sources follow a relatively rigid judgement pattern in the case of all individuals. The results of this judgement process agree to a large degree ' so that it is probable that the deter- minants of this process have similar interpersonal structures. With respect to the question regarding the future procedure to be adopted for risk-handling,the uniformity of response-behaviour is less pronounced between questionnaires 1 and 2. The correlation coefficients for the 13 risk sources vary from 0.13 (motor car) to 0.77 (heroIn). This may be due, on the one hand, to the fact that the individual questions as well as the response categories were phrased differently. On the other hand, it is also possible that . risk level and benefit receive the same interpersonal - 146 estimate, but that this does not apply to the desired consequences. If we use the separate estimates of benefits and detri- ments as a basis for analysis, we encounter a reciprocal relationship' the greater the benefit estimate, the lower will be the estimate of the detrimental effects. Both quantities correlate at an intensity of -0.96. This result is contrary to the values obtained in the Fischhoff study ('78, 114, p. 132 ff.) and an IAEA study by Otway ('77, 297, p. 15) where benefit and detriment were given different weights for overall judgement. However, different methodologies were used for these two studies. In spite of this discrepancy, a glance at Fiqure 15 reveals that, considering the point aggregation, the American values reported by Fischhoff have a structure which is highly similar to that of the German values. On the Whole there i. merely a parallel shift toward the left-hand bottom corner. This may be due to the linear transpOSition of the American data into the existing coordinate system or to the use of a different measuring technique. For FisChhoff et al divided their test subjects into two groups, one of which evaluated only the benefits and the other only the risks. ' Yet there is great agreement. both the relative spac- ing of the jIOints and their distance from the respective regression , line are .almost identical. This observation once again supports the theSis of,. a relatively universal estimation of "risk iUld ,benefit levels of known risk sources. Eatlmatlon of Benetlt X-Ro, Hom. AppU.nc •• MotorV.hlcle Cool Automation at Work Motor Vehlcl. Alrcroft Nuel •• , Energy SkIIng P •• tlcld •• X-Ro, Alrcroft H_ Appllonco. P •• tlcld •• Nuel •• r Energy Alcohol Alcohol Smoking Smoking Heroin Estimation of Risk German Data American Data - 14~ - Effect of Ideas on the Chllracterhtics of the Individual Risk Sources (Belief System) Two 1mportant results of the risk perception analysis must be retained so far: Risk estimation is only slightly dependent on the perceived loss rate per unit of tima. The evaluation of risk sources on the basis of their intuitive benefit-detriment distribution or risk- benefit estimate is relatively universal for all the interview subjects. The present chapter is concerned with describing a class of variables which might provide an explanation of the level and uniformity of risk-benefit estimates of differ- ent risk sources. the subjective ideas and arguments given for each risk source (the Bo-called belief system). For this purpose, a special scale including 46 statements on economic, health, personal, society-related and poli- tical aspects has been designed, and the interview sub- jects were requested to state the probability for the occurrence of each of these consequences.. Contrary to the otherwise customary Bcales, positive and negative consequences were measured separately to acquire the ranking of each statement with respect to both content (right or wrong) .and direction (encountered more frequent- ly AS a negative consequence or encountered more frequent- ·ly as a positive consequence). Fina~ly, similar to Fischbein's attitude measurement model (where the scale values tor the beliet system are multiplied by the gener- al subjective evaloation of these statements), the responses to each statement were weighted with the values ' of the so-called evaluation Bcale (the personal signifi- Cance ot each argument to the interview subject). However, due belief scale 150 - to reasons of methodology, the non-weighted was used predominantly .for evaluation. Complex statistical techniques had to be used for eval- uation of thes~ scales in order to ensure a meaningful and convincing reduction of the total of 1,196 variables to major parameters and factors . For this purpose, three statistical test procedures were used consecutively . - multiple regression of beliefs, using each risk- benefit est1mate and each evaluation index as depen- dent variables, in order to exclude irrelevant state- ments, factorial analysis of the remaining statements to reveal the basic structure and acquire the major factors I - -variance and reqre88ion analyses of thea. factor., ;using the risk perception variables which must be explained. The schematic in .Figure ·16 showa the results of this evaluation • .. This graph demonstrates the aignificance of the ·individual.belief factors tor the affective' evaluation of each risk source. TIle evaluation VUiable for each risk source must be seen separately 'from the variable of risk- benefit estimation. _,· It · was . determined as . the sum ot a so-called ' semantic 'differential, a technique which . has been tried 'and tested .in the .aocial sciences .as a valid instrument ' for the "acquisition of . aUecUve represent- ation of objects :. or>ogroupS .of penons In Qne's perceptive !.mages. The numerical.data on the ordinate represent the value of the declared variance .(R~) ' !i for the ,dependent variable "risk. source evaluat1on~ . ! - 151 - A number of interpretation possibilities exist for a comparison of the columns for the 12 risk types. The following relationships appear to have special interest: A common characteristic of the positive leaders in risk-benefit evaluation is that subject- related values and advantages play a dominant role (electrical appliances, X-rays, motor car, coal). The risk sources given a relatively positive evalua- tion (intermediate position) are characterized by strong emphasis on benefits to society and to the economy which have only minor effects on the individ- ual (aircraft, automation). Risk sources given a highly ambivalent evaluation (nuclear power stations, pesticides) are associated predominantly with socio-political and society- related disadvantages, while health aspects and per- sonal economic advantages are approximately balanced. In the case of risks given a clearly negative evalu- ation (smoking, alcohol, heroin) the aspect of the individual's own health detriment becomes largely dominant. This means that subject-referred values have a strong effect on risk-benefit estimation in both the positive and the negative range. The accep- tance of habit-forming products,in particular, appears to be dependent on the ratio of the perceived enjoy- ment factor to the perceived potential health detri~ ment. While the enjoyment factor of alcohol ranges one percentage point above the health detr~nt factor, the evaluation is inverted in the case of nicotine, and for heroin the ratio is 1.5. The riak- benefit estimates are given in the same negative sequence. This might be a useful fact"" for instance - 152- for public information campaigns. Health detriments are perceived for all habit-forming products, but their estimation and acceptance is siqnificantly determined by the ratio of perceived potential enjoy- ment and beUeved deqree of potential health detriment , regardless of the absolute level of the ccrresponding values . The skiing risk fall. slightly out of the general pattern: positive and personal aspects by far pre- dominate, but the risk-benefit a more intermediate position. thi. reflects symbolic beliefs estimation occupies It i. possible that not taken into consid- eration, such as "skiing is only for the affluent". Last but not least, the behavioural role component (where the interView subjects were skiers themselves) is likely to have a significant effect on the per- ception of skiing risk. - Another interesting area is the comparison between the alternatives of coal and nuclear energy. Both have the same purpose, the generation of electric power . However , the perception structure for these two ~ystema differs greatiy. Apparently coal is directly, asso,ciated with electric P>wer supply and hence with ci,uality of life, pleasure and enjoyment, whi,l" in" ~e case of nuclear energy the society- related, and, social constraints appear to be the mest,', important, linka in the association chain. The per~ived pers~nai advantages of coal energy over- shadow the also perceived disadvantages to the environ- men t and to !leu t1i. In the case of nuclear energy" health aspects and accident risks are a~ao found ,a. import~t negative :tnerefore, the predoounance of' negative items. Muftt .... It' .-... A_ ROokS- t p.,.on.' H •• lth 2 PI •• lur. end Comrort :I Socl.1 Burden 4 Oener.1 H •• lth - .... er ..... 5 SocI.1 Aetr09r .... on • P.r.onaID.pend.nc. 7 Pe~.IAd.a~ • Acckient RtaII; , .. tickle, "- ...... u."u. • ".tenanceor """It Condttlon to o-eral H •• lth t t Per.onll DI,.dvanta, •• t 2 EI'IvIrOftm.ntlH •• lth HtIde., .-.. Coal Motor Vehk:" Aln:ta" AutOM.t;lon .t; Work 13 Economic Adv.nt •••• 14 Sociopolitical Dian .. nt .... 1 5 Socia' Progr ••• 1. P.rlOtt.1 .nd Economic Adv.nt •••• , 7 Futur. Quality of Llf. t 8 Rlak or Llf. and limb 1 9 Environmental Hazarda 20 Accld.nt Rlak 21 Pr ••• nt~Day and Economic Technology - 155 - perception contents may be partly offset by pro- jected expectations in the future and advantages to society and to the economy. In addition to the individual interview data the medians for each risk source were also analysed as basic data for an aggregate evaluation in order to ensure still better comparability of the risk sources. This procedure was probably used in the study by Fischhoff et al. ('79, 112) but using geometric means, and approximately simulated in the IAEA studies by using three-dimensional factor analysis. However, the following must be kept in mind for this type of aggregate evaluation: the greater part of the variance is arbitrarily excluded; excessively high coefficients occur in correlation analyses/ factor analys~s will necessarily yield an identi- cal set of factors for each risk source. Of course, the last-named item describes a desirable result, that, unlike the graph presentation in Figure 1', the information contained in the different scales for each risk source can be combined into absolutely identical factors and that the values in each instance are comparable. However, this is an arbitrary homoge- nization procedure whose results can be interpreted only with reservations . This type of factor analysis of the medians yielded the following basic dimensions: -156 - consequences for the individual (pleasure, happiness, health, leisure, los,es, ~Qnyenlenc •• education), effects on welfare and society financial (~ality of life, supply, general health, protection of life, standard of living, security, aocial justice, conservation of the environment); social and technological progress or retrogression (modernization, progress of society, advantages to future generations)r socio-political values (freedom, progress, democratic rights, citizen's participation, social justice), direct personal advantages and disadvantages (plea- sure, personal advancement, happiness, convenience). Figures 17a to l1d represent the content structure of the aggregate data. A presentation similar to that of the IAEA study (cf. Page 34 ) was selected. The five factors are plotted as block diagrams and, therefore, allow direct viaual comparison for each risk source. Again, the three habit-forming products exhibit the sequence of alcohol-smoking-heroin in almost all evaluation asPects; The greateat personal advantagea are "i,sociate,!" wi th alcohol, followed by Cigarette smok- ing and heroi.n; The same appliea approximately to the perception of SOCiety-related consequences. While the social progress factor has no interpretative signifi- cance (as alreadY'"descr1bed in the case" of the indivi- dual analysis), alcohol and" smoking are " reversed for the socio-political effects. Although the difference Alcohol Smoking 81 - Pe_nol Direct ondlndlrect Adyonteg .. ond D_yonteg .. 82 - Eflecte for lho Public end the Society ·83 - 8ocIoiend Technologlcol Prog_ (Retrog,...lon) 84 - 8oclopolltlcol Volue. al - Peraon •• Convenience .net Pleeeure Heroin "l ..... lQ ~ -..J tr tr X 0 :t' I ..... lQ :0 lQ '" "l '1 '< '" ro cn Cl lQ rt '" 0 rt t'l '1 ro I-' ..... ro '" tIl Cl I-' ro rt I-' '1 t'" ..... I-'- 0 ro Cl '" ..... '" 0- I-' cn Ul rt :t' '1 '0 C '0 Cl I-' rt ..... C '" '1 ::l ro Cl ro "l cn '" Cl rt Ul 0 ;..- '1 ..... cn ..... ::l lQ Ul ~ '" rt I-'- 0 ::s • 51 . 52 0 53 .54 . 55 Röntgen elektr. Haushaltsgeräte persönliche direkte und indirekte Vor- und Nachteile qesellschaftli('he und soziale Vor- und Nachteile sozialer und technologischer Fortschritt (Rückschritt) sozia lpolitische Argumente direkte persönliche Vor- und Nachteile Skifahren ..... (J1 '" less acceptable until a deliberately negative relation- ship to risk on the whole exists, technological or industrial risks, on the other hand, must already prove their justification when no positive attitude toward risk-taking exists. However, this inverted relation does not apply to heroin which is given such a high negative evaluation by all the interview subjects that significant correlations cannot exist at all. However, the limits of interpretation capabilities must be pointed out once again: high correlation on the risk propensity scale merely means that interview sub- jects who gave very high or very low evaluations to 11 risk sources also did this consistently in the case of the twelfth source. Whether this trend toward con- sistency actually reflects the dispositive characteris- tic of risk propensity, the degree to which this is merely verbal, but not actual behaviOur, whether some persons do not have different matrices and evaluation patterns for numerical values, cannot be clarified by the present study. Effect of Demographic and Social Characteristics Some demographic and social characteristics were also investigat~ as part of the risk perception study. Again, a few simple correlation analyses to determine the correlations with risk evaluation and risk-benefit - estimation can reveal the effect of this variable. The coefficients have been compiled in Table 13. First, it is clearly seen that political party preference does not play any role, with the exception of nuclear energy risk estimation. That nuclear energy would be 191 - more likely to receive a positive evaluation by conservative voters (Christian Democratic Party) and a negative evaluation by socialist party voters (Social Democratic Party) could have been expected on the basis of the most recent opinion polls, a detailed discussion will follow in Part V. But the variables of sex, age and clas8 are of greater interest. This applies especially to pes- ticides, where women, younger persona and members of the upper classes are more likely to give negative evaluations. With the exception of class membership, the same applies to nuclear energy. In either case it i. likely that the "artificial nature" of the ri.k source, the involuntary risk-taking which cannot be controlled by the individual and the possibility of cataatrophic detriments have a greater repulsive effect on women and younger people. Even at this early time the prediction appears justified that pesticides and - through them - all chemical interventions in the food chain, from the point of view of the overall ri.k per- ception structure will encounter the same acceptance problema from which nuclear energy suffers today, al- though the topic may not be as controversial because the specific risk-benefit distribution for nuclear en- ergy is still further removed from the congruent model than it 18 in the case of pesticides. The sex variable also affects the risk estimation for aircraft, alcohol (but not smoking) and X-ray diagno.- tics. Again, only speculative explanation patterns can be given. It ia possible that a fear of radiation which Cannot be perceived by the sensory organs and a apecial sensibility with respect to health hazards are deCisive for the negative evaluation of X-ray diagnos- tic. by women; it is likely that women .uffer from alcohol abuse by their mates more frequently than vice 192 - versa, and it is a known fact from the psychology of air traffic that women suffer greater anxieties prior to flying than men (which, however, is not yet an explanation of this phenomenon). The degree to which socialization-related factors (learning to express feelings), role-specific patterns (women are responsible for family, health and food) or even endogenous phenom- ena (hormone changes, pregnancy) are potential influenc- ing parameters, would be an interesting problem for future studies. Age- or class-specific relationships are less clearly developed. Nicotine and alcohol are given a more negative evaluation by the members of the upper clasle., although they do not demand more stringent measures against them. Automation on the job exhibits a slightly positive correlation with class and a slightly negativs correlation with age. contrary to the intuitive assump- tion that it is especially the older employees who feel insecure because of the introduction of modern machinery on the job, it is especially the younger employees with low occupational prestige who perceive the automation risk to be negative. Another surprising feature is the greater risk estimation for X-ray diagnostics given by young people: it is possibla that this discrepancy is due to better information on potential radiation hazard. on the part of the younger generation. Finally, it shOUld be mentioned that younger persons and members of higher classes are more likely to vote again.t a prohibition of heroin (and instead would prefer volun- tary abstinence), and that both types of persons would also prefer more stringent measures against nuclear energy. 193 - The comprehensive preparation for the present study in 1978 by accident yielded an interesting result. In the factor analysis of the evaluation scale, i.e. the EiRbbein weighting factors (64 items) for the belief scale, three factors were revealed as signifi- cant dimensions which could be labelled as follows . - direct, subject-related advantages and disadvantages (more benefits, greater income etc.): - indirect, subject- and group-related advantages and disadvantages (health, education etc.), - society-related and socio-political advantages and disadvantages (more democratic rights, economic advantages etc . ). The subsequent correlation of these factors with class membership revealed a clear-cut relationship which haa been plotted in Figure 22. This shows that the lower class attributes major weight to direct advantages, the lower middle class to indirect advantages and the upper middle class to the society aspects, while the upper class aSSigns the same weight to all three factors. This result,which reveals class-specific weighting patterns for argumentation levels,was already presented aBa thesis at the Miami Energy Forum in 1978 (Engelmann, Rann, '80, 96, p. 364). In the survey itself, using 100 interview subjects each and a stricter belief scale , more differentiated claas- Specific differences in weighting behaviour were re- vealed; Similar to the factorial analysis of the aggre- gate belief scale, the analysis of the Fishbein weights (evaluation scale) revealed five factors . I~ - direct and indirect effects on health, life, happiness and security; economy- and society-related values such as quality of life, supply, social justice, environmental pollution; projective and political advantages and disadvantages such as modernization, advantages or disadvantages to future generations, progress of society, democrat- ic rights; socio-political values such as freedom, participation by citizens, education, social justice; direct personal advantages and disadvantages such as pleasure, financial advantages, convenience, happi- ness. Pigure 2l shows the relative significance of these five factors for each class category. Similar to the pre- liminary studies, the significance of subject-related aspects declines with class level, but without leading to any differentiation between direct or indirect con- sequences. At the same time, the importance of society- and economy-related values rises with the class index on the average. Projective advantages and disadvantages are given a higher evaluation from the lower middle class up, but the differences are relatively small here. Also, political aspects such as freedom and participation by the citizens are evaluated a8 important only from this class level, and the proportion of this factor rises continuously until the upper class is reached. The lower cla8s and ~ to a slightly lesser degree - the upper cla88 attribute more weight to direct, personal advantage8 such as pleasure and convenience than the ~ middle classes. " Low Lower Stratum High Lower Stratum direct benefit/coat Indirect benefit/coat eltruletlc benefit/coat Low Middle Stratum High Middle Stratum High Stratum ("I Low Lower Stratum High Lower Stratum Pereonal Adventages/Pleasure (Factor 5) Sociopolitical Values (Factor 4) Low Mlddla Stratum High Middle Stratum High Stratum Projected Consequencles/Polltlcal Values (Factor 3) Economic and Social Values (Factor 2) Personal Direct and Indirect Advantages (Factor 1) A detailed interpretation of these class-specific evaluation patterns would go beyond the bounds of this study. Thus, it could be discussed in detail whether the middle class,which is especially receptive to career stimuli, deliberately reduces the importance of aspects reflecting a positive attitude toward life, such as pleasure and convenience (in the form of sublimation), or whether it has internalized the ascetic way of life in the sense of Max Weber's theories on the relationships between capital- ist. advancement and anti-consumption values. Also in reference to participation research, the question con- cerning the degree to which demands for democratic participation rights, citizen's participation and political freedom are not typical interests of an upper class which is privileged in any event, would have to be clarified. In the present study these questions can only be mentioned briefly. Overall Analysis of the Factors Influencing the Perception and Acceptance of Risks Up to this point, the analysiS of the results on risk perception was limited to a gradual incorporation of the relevant claaaes of variables: first, the specific beliefs on each risk source, subsequently the qualita- tive characteristics and finally the external factors were discussed. In addition, evaluation of the seman- tic differential whose summated value was used aa tha indicator for risk source evaluation revealed several additional influencing factors which were labelled as follows: - security, - aCientificality, - analogue classification (auch aa sympathetic, ethical), - POWer. 200 In order to determine the ranking of each of these factors for the evaluation of the risk source. a multiple regression was made by introducing all the relevant factors of the different classes of variables and the demographic and social characteristics as independent parameters and determining their relative weights on the basis of the values for the explained variance of the two dependent variables (risk-benefit estimation and risk source evaluation). However. this type of overall statistical evaluation must be inter- preted with special caution since common interaction effects between the variables are taken into considera- tion but their internal. causal structure is concealed. In particular. it should be pointed out that social structure data such as age and class have a quite dif- ferent relationship to the dependent variable than. for instance. the different qualitative characteristics which in turn can depend on the aocial data. The six belief factors. the five most important quali- tative characteristics (not summarized) and five of the six factors of the semantic differential (but only for the regression of the risk-benefit estimation). exclud- ing the "general evaluation" factor. and the external variables of "dispositive risk propensity" and social structure data were selected as independent variables for each risk source. The total number of variables was 32 since dummy variables were uaed for the data which were not subjected to interval-scaling (sex. pol- itical party preference). As in the preceding chapter •• the risk-benefit estimation and the sum indices for the semantic differential served as i!ependent variables. The individual classes of variables are colour-coded in Figures 24& to 24e (green • social and demographic characteristics 1 bl~.e • beUef scale factors, red .. u_, Con"QulneIN ,--NIIk .. u. ... ~ VA: tIf, fttIIk -. ""',; ........ _ ..... ........ Attanwtt,. AlQIMr, ..... u .... ~'1.,.... Rlek-Benefit &tlmetlon Dlapoeltlve and External Factor. F actore of the Belief - Syetem Qualitative Risk - and Beneflt-Properties F actore of the Semantic Differential Heroin A".Of"~t Soc:la' Risk, u_, "-'"'" Volont.ry RIIIk hneflt Oriented Per.onal D~nd.I't(:Y Willing to Take RIIk. All Oth.r Factor, UM.~ V.-nc. Semantic Differential All Other Facto", Accounting lor Le .. Than Two Percent of the Explained Variance - 211 - qualitative characteristics, and yellow. semantic differential factors) so that the lignificance of these classes becomes evident at first glance. The first striking feature is the low explained value of the semantic differential factors, where the basic factor, general evaluation, had of course not been included. Factor 2 of the semantic differential, lecurity and risk, plays a role only in the caae of coal, nuclear energy and electrical appliances, while it doe. not appear at all in the case of the habit-formin9 products . But, belief scale facto... of similar content are represented in the case of smoking, heroin, pesticides, X-rays and motor car., 80 that it is a nat"ral assumption that then factors have better correlation with the dependent variables, thUB, they con- ceal the information content of the semantic differ- ential factors which have a similar con- tent, so that they no lonqer yield any additional explained values. In the case of the qualitative riak-benefit character- istics, the previously obaerved trend ia confirmed that risk-benefit estimation or risk Bource evaluation are affected only when certain threshold values have been exceeded. Thus, it 1a Been in the CAse of the hablt- fOrming products that a clear emphasi8 exist. on the voluntary nature of risk-takin9, and in the case of nuclear energy and pesticides that there is a strong wei9htinq in terms of the type and severeness of ri.k consequences. Also, the individual controllability component in the case of the motor car i8 more clearly expresed in this final analysis than it was in the indi- vidual evaluations. On the other hand, the benefit com- ponent which dominated in the analyais of qualitative ifi ce probably due characteristics decline. in si,.n can , already covered to the fact that distribution aspects are by the statements on the belief acale. - 212 - The belief 8cale factor8 and dispo8itive characteri8tics or social data (blue, green) account for the greatest proportion of the explained value. And the ri8k evalua- tion values can claim special validity because the in- ternally acquired variables of risk propen. i 1., and benefit detriment orientation are genuinely independent . What are the conclusions which can be drawn for the individual risk sources? In the case of smoking and heroin the belief factor8 of health, risk to life and plealure are predominant. The effect of general benefit-or-detriment orientation 18 le88 developed. In the caae of nuclear energy and pesticides, risk propensity appears to be a necessary prerequisite for positive evaluation. In both case8, this variable plays a primary role. Socio-political and security-specific problema as well a8 the qualitative risk characteristics of -delayed and catastrophic con- sequences· take second place. The positive aspects of benefits, supply, economic and projective advantage I are characterized by low variance components only. On the other hand, belief in possible personal advantages and consequences to health dominate in the cale ·of coal and X-rays. Theae influence an subject-related variable. have a decisive attitude. toward these risk 80urces. Ri8k propensity is a180 an important explanation factor in the case of motor cara and automation. Persona who exhibit a high verbal risk propenai.ty apparently tran8- fer thi. attitude especially to motor car. and automation and, to a Ilightly le88er degree, to pesticides and nuclear energy, while persons of greater risk-8cepticism qive these risk sources an excessively negative evalua- tion. - 213 - The positive and negative belief factors are approxi- mately balanced in the case of automation and motor cars. Economic and personal advantages are in favour of technology on the job and , at the lame time, the perception of aocial di8advantagel and the health hazard play a role a8 well. The motor car is aS80ci- ated with the fulfillment of personal desirea and values, but alao with pollution and accident risk aa negative effects . Benefit-orientation i& of primary significance for the evaluation of electrical appliance., meaning that per- aona who, on the whole, consider all riak sources from the point of view of their advantages, tend especially to tranafer this attitude to electrical home appliances. In addition, the factors of voluntarines8, economic advantages and progress to society have an effect. The presentation of the different component. of the explanation level in the form of circular legmenta demonstrates that the belief structure, the qualitative risk-benefit characteristics, some dispositive charac- teristic. and - to a lesser degree - semantic differen- tial factors can be used to explain approximately 50 to 60' of the variance encountered in the risk-benefit estimations and in the .um index of the semantic dif- ferential. Figure. 24& to 248 al.o demonstrate the weaknesses of aggregate analyses. new, hardly typlflable explanation pattern. result for each risk source which in themselVes are quite plausible but can certainly not be reduced to general structures which can be qeneralized tor risk acceptance. It we introduce aggregate data into the analyais, we create not only an aztit1cially homogenized - 214 - framework 80 that the different risk sources can be viewed through standardized spectacles, we also obtain rela- tive values over 0.90 which are not confirmed by the individual analys1s, or only to a much lesser degree. Impressive proof for this data manipulation can be found through a regression of the aggregate influencing factors for all 12 risk sources. Compared to the cor- relation values to which we have become accustomed in the soclal sciences, Table 14 reveals a truely sensAtion- al reBult: Table 14: Aggregate Regression Analysis for the Sum Index of the Semantic Differential Summated Declared Additional Simple Independent variable correlation variance explanation correlation value S 6 (summated beliefs) S 5 (personal advantages and dis- advantages) 0 2 (risk dis- tributiotl1 0 1 (voluntari- nes8) 0 12 (no alter- natives) o 8 (minor con- sequences) 0.959 0.974 0.986 0.992 0.999 1.00 0.92 0.95 0.974 0.98 0.998 1.00 , 0.92 0.03 0.03 0.01 0.01 0.001 0.96 0.86 0.96 -0.30 -0.38 0.47 With the aid of six variables, the median of the indepen- dent v~rlable, risk evaluation, can be predicted to an accu- Tacy of two decimal places. This type ,of determiniatic relationshlp 1s ~arely f~und even in the natural sclences. Aa much as we might be pleased by this clear-cut and satisfying result, we must conSider it A product of the - 215 _ statistical procedure used where the greater part of the non-clarified variance has been excluded by form- ing the mean-values, sinoe the individual data were aggregated into nine cases (risk Boureea representing the individual case) . If we neglect, for the moment, the quantitative value. and study only the influence chain as Buch, the results of these regressions can be interpreted in a qulte meaningful way. The most important influencing factor is the sum index of the belief scale, followed by the perceived subjec- tive advantages and disadvantages (which 1n part are already contained in the sum index). The third place i. occupied by risk distribution, again an indication of the siqnificance of this factor to risk perception, followed by the qualitative risk characteristics of ·voluntarlne88, no alternatives possible and minor con- 8equences~. If we use only the aggregated value., we can draw the conclusion that a risk Bource which 1_ characterized by an overall positive belief structure, by perceived subjective advantages, by voluntary riak- taking, by an equal distribution of benefits and risk., by the absence of catastrophiC effects and by the ab- sence of any alternative of equivalent benefits, 1s accepted without any problem. This rule provides little information on the riak sources whose acceptance 1s subject to conflict: 1n the case of nuclear energy and pesticides as well as the habit- forming products, specifiC ri,k perception pattern, occur, auch as the socia-political aspects, each of Which have an effect on the risk-taking decision, but Without contributing to the explanation model on the aggregate level. At best, this six-variable model can be used as a starting point for risk evaluation, pro- Vided that the specific characteristics of the ri'k -m- source. 1n question are al.o analysed 1n detail. To that extent. a possibility develops in principle whereby a global model can first be used for a rough acceptance determination. to be followed by individual analyses to clarify the degree to which special factors in the different factor cla •• es influence the empirical acceptance by the population. - 217 _ Part V Nuclear Energy Attitudes and their Determinants - Results of Sampling Surveys - 218 - Post-War Nuclear Energy Attitudes of the Population of the Federal Republic of Germany Since the early fifties, nuclear energy and nuclear power stations have been popular topics for commercial and academic opinion polling. While, in the fifties and sixties, public interest was centered on nuclear weapons applications, interest shifted to the peaceful uses of nuclear energy in power-generating reactors toward the mid-sixties. A brief historical description of the different phases of nuclear energy attitude formation is useful for following the trend of public opinion-forming over time (cf. Table 15). As far as the fifties are concerned, American studies must be used predominantly (Erskine, '63, 102, p. 180 ff.,; Douvan and Withey' 54, 82, p. 1 ff-; Levine and Modell' 65, 221, p. 275 ff.; Fischer et al, '51,113, p. 86ff;Back and Gergen '63, 20, p. 428 ff.) since German polling institutions did not conduct major nuclear energy surveys until 1969. Phase I: Until apprOXimately 1950/51, there was an ambivalent attitude toward nuclear energy: on the one hand, there was the fear of the horrors of nuclear Warfare, and on the other hand the acknowledgement that nuclear weapons were significant to ensure the military superi- ority of one's own country. During this pe- riod, the peaceful Use of nuclear energy plays no role at all and is either not perceived, or only vaguely so (Erskine, '63, 102, p. 180; Paschen, '78, 307, p. 3; Bieber, '77, 33, p. 3 ff;; Renn, '77, 331, p. 7 ff.). Phase II: During the fifties, there was a clear teversal of public opinion against nuclear weapons which found an expression in the Easter marches. - 219 - A nuclear weapons stop was demanded world- wide, and nuclear fall-out was considered a threat to one's own health, especially in the United States. The Atoms for Peace movement, initiated by President Eisenhower, created a counter-movement to the military threat and hopes for a reversal of man's destructive intellectual potential into a harnessing of natural resources for humane purposes. This metaphysical embellishment of nuclear energy, promoted for political reasons, prepared the first step toward a symbolic role of nuclear energy as a paragon of technology and innovation (Douvan and Withey, '54, 82, p. 2; Rosi, '65, 345, p. 290 ff.;Levine, Modell, '65, 221, p. 275/ Hlifele, '75, 154, p. 44 ff.;Renn, '77, 331, p. 11 ff.) Phase III: When the test stop agreement was signed between the United States and the USSR, the spearhead of protest against nuclear weapons was broken; nuclear weapons took second place after the problems of substitute wars (such as Vietnam). The theory of the arms balance also gained ground among the population and, in addition, the re- nunciation of nuclear bomb tests in the atmos- phere reduced the general anxiety threshold. The 'question of peaceful uses still had little relevance. While there were still protests against the construction of research reactors in the fifties, where the typical characteris- tics of traditional technological adaptation problems existed, the first commercial nuclear power stations could be commissioned in the late sixties without any appreciable protest or resistance (Bieber, '77, 33, p. 82 ff.; Schuster, '71, 403, p. 113 ff.; Erskine, 102, p. 162 ff./ Renn, '77, 331, p. 9 ff.) - 220 - Table 15: Popular Opinions on Nuclear Energy Over Time (1945 - 80) Phases Years Attitude to- Attitude to- Knowledqe Polari- ward nuclear ward peace- level zation weapons ful uses I 40-50 slightly neq- not yet low hardly ative, but differen- any acknowledqe- tiated ment of the nuclear bal- ance of forces II 51-62 very neqative sl1qhtly low greater positive, for weap- but still ons appl1- not very cations relevant III 63-69 fairly neu- fairly better, none traI, less poSitive, but still relevant hardly neg- low at1ve, hiqh degree of indifference IV 69-7.5 no longer as qreat major- fairly greater relevant as ity poSitive, adequate before, ex- 20-301 neqa- cept for t1ve, 10-151 . prOliferation indifferent fears V 76-78 no longer 301 POSitive, relatively qreat relevant 25' negative, high 45' ambiva- lent VI 79-80 reqains pub- dualism be- relatively great, but lic influence ween per- hiqh weakening ceived inse- in confron- curity and belief in tadon the necessity of nuclear energy - 221 - Phase IV: In the late sixties and early seventies, the criticism of peaceful nuclear energy uses which had been aroused in the United States was seized upon in the Federal Republic of Germany. The problems were first discussed among scientists and only then presented to the public by professional critics. At the same time, local resistance to the construction of nuclear facilities developed. None of this would have had the result that nuclear energy grew into a major political problem if the symbolic nature of nuclear en- ergy which had developed in the fifties had not been seized again and reinterpreted. The developing awareness of the limits of growth, the greater interest in post-material values on the part of younger people with university education, the greater sensitization of the population for environmentalism and nature, and the disappointed hopes aeto the effects of techno- logical and scientific progress (such as space flight or cancer research) had the result that, in the minds of many individuals and groups, the symbolic attributes of nuclear energy, such a8 "progressive, clean, centralized and complex", turned into a negative connotation. In spite of the first opposition fronts against nuclear energy, there was initially a solidarity with pro-nuclear energy groups in the Federal Republic of Germany, most of which were close to the established institutions in politics and society (cf. Table 16). Opinion polls confirmed (cf. Allensbach, Table 33 and Bieber, '77, 33, p. 87) that the number of strong and moderate - 222 - advocates of nuclear energy rose to approxi- mately 70t, while less than one tenth of the population considered themselves opponents of the new technology. This surprising fact can be partly explained by the energy crisis in 1973 where nuclear energy was perceived as an escape from the ,011 dilemma e Moreover I the persons who were opponents of nuclear energy in the fifties and sixties (characteristics: conservative, advanced Age, low specific know- how, fear of losing social status) failed to find a social equivalent in the new protest group against nuclear facilities (persons of younger age with university education and a more leftist political trend) and, therefore, fe·lt insecure for political reasons. On the other hand, the protest movement was not yet strong enough to convince the potential adher- ents of the new innovation protest (Paschen, '78, 307, p. 5 ff.; Bieber, '77, 33, p. 85 ff.; Renn, '78, 332, p. 10 ft.). Phase V: The characteristic feature of Phase V which started approximately with the protest activities in the Ge~an town of Wyhl (19.75) was the increasing consolida- tion of nuclear energy opponents. Support by persons engaged in science and technology, the popular base in the local environment, the organization of citizens' initiatives and the reactions on the part of official politics which were clumsy, to say the . least, had the result that more and more citizens questioned the motives of nuclear poliey and adopted sceptical evaluations. Also, doubts as to the relevance of t he nuclear programme were increasingly reported in the media. - 223 - Table 16: Nuclear Energy Attitudes in the Federal Republic of Germany (Summary of Various Polls) 1) 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 7) Posi- '75 Jan. '76 Dec.'76 Mid '77 End of May '78 Nov.' 78 tiona '77 Adher- 60 39 57 59 51 39 32 ents Oppon- 16 20 41 40 27 18 36 ents 1ndif- 24 30 3 1 22 43 32 ferent 8) 9) 10) 11) 12) 13) 14) Dec. '78 Apr.' 79 July '79 July' 79 Aug.' 79 Jan.' 80 June 'SO Hesse Hesse Adher- 40 50 61 52 37 56 ents Oppcn- 39 33 22 30 48 42 ents 1ndif- 21 12 17 18 15 2 ferent 1) Quoted from D. Goerke (Goerke, '75, 131, p. 1112) 2) Infas study, FRG, No. 1315, 1976 3) Sample Institut, Hamburg, source: letter to "Deutsches Atomforum", dated 4.2.77 67 32 2 4} Intermarket poll, source: Kernzeitung, New Information on Energy and Energy Policy, published by the 1nformations- kreis Kernenergle, only edition, Bonn 1977 5) Spiegel poll: Do We Need Nuclear Power? (Brauchen wir Atomkaft?), No.8, Hamburg '77, p. 163 6) DlGOE Marketing Service, Vechta, May '78 (Goerke, '78 •.. , p. 133) 7) Infra-Test, FRG, Nov. '78 8) Stern, 17 .07.79 9) F¥ankfurter Rundschau, 16.08.79, Infas-Hessen 10) Same a. footnote 9 11) Same as footnote 8 12) Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung, 8.08.79, Forschungsgruppe Wahlen, Mannheim 13) Der Spiegel, No. 19/1980, p. 44 14) Emnid 1980 - 224 • The discussion of the hazards of nuclear energy and some apocalyptic scenarios published by sceptics created a high potential of active resistance in the local environment of planned nuclear energy facilities which was discharged in ma&sivedemonstrations and sometimes even mili- tant activities (Brokdorf, Stade, Kalkar). On the interregional level, the symbolic notion of nuclear energy for the environmentalist movement and for the decentralization movement - politically enriched by the Nixon scandal in the United States - developed further, and a llvely exchange of activists in the new anti- nuclear power movement was initiated. Among the public this resulted in an increased perception of nuclear energy opponents who were ~egarded as being a majority. ~uring this phase, a structurized attitude formed in most individuals; approximately 25' were clear-cut opponents, approximately 30' adherents, and the remaining 45' were ambivalent between the two extremes depending on the issues of the day and the political mood. Only a small proportion of not more than 10' were really indifferent (cf. Table 16; Scharioth, Krebsbach, '77, 382, p. 3; Pas~ent '78, 307, p. 6 ff.; Renn, '78, 332, p. 108). - 225 - Phase VI: While the years from 1975 to 1978 were char- acterized by confrontation between opponents and adherents and the resulting polarization of views, the beginning of the Iran crisis and the Three Mile Island accident marked a trend toward less denunciation in the conflict and more attempts to find solutions that would allow political compromise. However, this should not be misunderstood as a levelling of positions. The fronts had neither softened, nor had they approached each other, but the conflicts had lost in virulence (partly due to lack of an oppar- tunity),and both camps were undergOing an inter- nal consolidation process to develop new argu- ments and new ideas for future conflicts. In the general publiC, the opinion structures which had developed in the mid-seventies were more or less re- tained. Following a brief opinion slump after Three Mile Island, an increasingly positive basic attitude in the question as to the necessity of nuclear energy developed again among the population. However, a number of contradictory results were obtained especially at the end of 1979 (for instance, between Infas and Studien- gruppe Wahlen of Mannheim). An interesting feature in this conjunction was the increasingly critical evalua- tion of the safety of nuclear facilities and their engineering maturity (reactions to the Three Mile Island accident) and the increasingly positive evaluation of their economic necessity. With this background on the social development of the nuclear energy conflict, the dat~ acquired in our studies can be better classLfied and understood. This empirical study to determine nuclear energy attitudes was carried out on the basis of population samples of five towns during the period from March to September 1979. These - 226 - towns were selected using the following criteria: _ regional comparability (all towns are located in the same federal state); _ experience with nuclear facilities or confrontation with the planned construction of such a facility (however, the fifth town has no relationship to nuclear energy and was included as the control). Table 17, below, lists the data for the popUla- tion's nuclear energy attitude. The indicator for the interview subject's attitude was his declared intention to vote pro- or contra- at a referendum on further utiliza- tion of nuclear energy. With the exception of Hamm, the majority in all five survey units was in favour of continuing the construction , of nuclear facilities; about one third was in favour of a construction stop, and one fifth would either not go to vote or was still undecided. Significant differences are found between the values for Hamm (many opponents) and the other towns, and between JUlich (particuldrly few opponents) and the other towns. Little change in these basic relations is noted when the interview subjects are asked for their attitude toward nuclear faCilities, or the planned construction of such faCilities, in the vicinity of their homes. The correlation coefficient between voting behaviour in a referendum and a specific vote on a nuclear facility located close to the subjects' home is 0.68 (gamma coefficient). The high level of agreement in the fre- quency distribution between these two variables may be considered evidence that a mentality which would approve nuclear power stations as long as they are not built in one's own vicinity, is not typical for the repondents under stUdy. - 227 - Table 17: Voting Behaviour in a Nuclear Energy Referendum Hunic!pal:ttleB Kerpen Jlllich Bever- """en Samm Kalkar n Characteristics All survey Control Nuclear Nuclear Nuclear Fast areas town research power power ata- breeder facility station tion under under con- construction etruction continued con- struction of 46 51 58 44 31 47 nuclear facili- ties construction stop for 33 37 21 35 39 30 nuclear facilities Undecided Would not to vote n 16 9 15 18 25 16 . go 5 3 6 2 5 7 503 120 100 99 100 . 84 Th.ls correspondence between gener,:ll attitude and local preference may be due to the fact that nuclear facilities already exist in four of these five survey areas, or that construction is in progress there, so that the existing reality has established a link between nuclear power stations in general and facilities located in the vicinity of the interview subjects' But the same parallelism of these two home. variables found in the present study was just as great for the control town of Kerpen (Spearman correlation index a 0.57) so that additional justification exists for speaking of a parallel trend in the evaluation of nuclear power stations far removed and close to one's home. This in- terpretation is further supported by the fact that, in a previous study, this author found a comparably high correlation between these two variables (Renn, '77, 331, p.SO) . The perception of the necessity of nuclear power facilities and the appreciation that this might affect one's own way of life, would,therefore,not appear to be contradictory. 232 166 80 25 503 - 228 - Effect of the Three Mile Island Reactor Accident on the population The most serious accident to date in the history of the peaceful use of nuclear energy, at least in the Western World, occurred en 28 March 1979 at the Three Mile Island Nuclear Power Station near Harrisburg, Pennsylvania (Kemeny Report, German edition, '80, 415, p. 18). This accident was initiated by mechanical malfunctions in the reactor and severely aggravated by a series of human errors made in attempts to correct these malfunctions. For days there were conflicting opinions as to whether an explosive oxygen-hydrogen mixture had developed and whether there was imminent danger to the population from explosions or from the emission of radioactive gases. Pregnant women were evacuated as a precautionary measure; thousands of citizens departed voluntarily from the area. Hardly any technological accident has ever triggered as broad a mass effect in the media as the Harrisburg accident. It is not the purpose of this study to describe or analyze the causes and consequences of this accident; rather, its object is to describe the reactions of the population to the Harrisburg accident. For this purpose, a description of the international reactions appears indicated first (cf. Table 18, below). - 230 - In the United States and Sweden (but also in The Nether- lands and Spain), there was a clearly negative shift in public opinion, while the drop in France and in the Federal Republic of Germany (but also in the United Kingdom and Switzerland) was limited and was recovered again in the course of the subsequent months. These different types of reaction to the Three Mile Island accident have not yet been given a theoretical explanation framework. The present study might yield sorne indications as to why the Three Mile Island accident caused such different reactions in different parts of the World. Due to the time phasing of the interviews conducted in JUlich the effect of this accident could be determined through comparison of the numerical data acquired prior to and after 28 March and, at the same time, several questions on the effects of the Three Mile Island accident were included in questionnaire No. III. The direct comparison shall be discussed first: Table 19: Three Mile Island Effect : Comparison of the ValUes for Nuclear Energy Attitudes Prior to and After the Accident (JUlich only) Attitude Pro nuclear energy Slightly positive Neutral Slightly negative Negative n Sig. - 0.151 Lambda = 0.13 Percentages Percentages prior to the accident after the accident 6.0 4.1 34.0 20.4 28.0 38.8 20.0 22.4 12 . 0 14.3 50 49 - 231 - Following the general trend in the Federal Republic of Germany, the purely quantitative effect of the Three Mile Island accident was relatively low in JUlich. Considering the small number of cases, the relation is not even signifi- cant. An interesting feature of this distribution is the relatively great rigidity of the extreme attitudes and the greater migration from the moderately positive to the neutral attitude. This impression becomes even stronger when the values for the direct question as to the effect of the ~eeMile Island accident are consulted. Table 20. Reactions to the Thr.ee Mile Island Accident (in percent) Effects of the accident all towns Kerpen JdUch Beverungen Bamm ICalkar Positive attitude 1 3 0 1 1 2 con£lrme4 Opinion not influenced 22 19 21 26 18 27 Merely stimu- lated interest 33 36 39 30 34 21 Slightly more negative 27 27 31 21 28 25 attitude Much more negative 8 8 3 9 6 12 attitude Previously negative attitude 10 6 6 12 12 12 confirmed Median 3. 3 3.3 3.3 3.3 3.4 3.4 In the case of more than SS percent of the interview subj~cts the Three Mile Island accident did not have any attitude-modifying effect; almost every third interview subject stated a slightly more negative attitude, and only one out of twelve subjects felt that this accident had caused a major attitude change. Again, marginal effects between neighboring - 232 - forms of an attitude are found more frequently than a com- plete deviation from a previously held opinion. In addition, subdivision of the reactions between nuclear energy adherents and opponents allows an estimate of the quantitative effects of the opinion change. Approximately 10 , of the opponents stated that the accident had induced in them a much more negative attitude toward nuclear energy. The data for the national poll conducted by the Institut fUr Demoskopie in Allensbach are in the same order of magnitude. A more detailed study of our survey data revealed, moreover, that interview subjects claiming a large shift of attitude after the reactor accident gave especially inconsistent replies, had not developed a very strong position and exhibited below-average values on the knowledge scale. This leads to the natural assumption that persons who were concerned only marginally with the nuclear energy topiC prior to the Three Mile Island accident developed especially critical attitudes toward nuclear energy as a result of the extensive reporting on the accident without, however, having developed the strength of argument to support this opinion. The subsequent polls showed that, over time, some of the negative shift in the attitude of these persons was eliminated again. Interview subjects who had already developed a positive or negative attitude prior to the accident were given only slight cause to doubt their basiC attitudes. However, this evaluation of the Three Mile Island accident applies only to the general attitude on nuclear energy, not to the evaluation of the nuclear energy risk, where the available data indicate a greater shift towards negative evaluations. On the whole, the results of data analysis can be summarized to the effect that the variable consolidation of attitudes was responsible for the intensity at the Three Mile Island effect - 233 - and that it failed to influence firmly established decisions on nuclear energy (invaccination effect), but that it did influence elements of these attitudes (such as the risk apprecia- tion). Ih:.response to the Three Idj 1e Is land aceident the nuclear energy community frequently argues that the special safety pre- cautions taken in German nuclear power stations are per- ceived even by the public to be so exemplary that accidents of the Three Mile Island type cou~d not occur in this country. This hypothesis, too, was tested in the survey. In WUrgassen, Hamm and Ka1kar the nuclear power stations existing or 1n the process of construction were used for comparison, and in JU1ich and Kerpen the German nuclear power stations on the whole. The results of these interviews showed that most interview subjects evaluated the safety of German nuclear power stations to be neither poorer nor better than the safety of the American reactor on Three Mile Island. Almost half the interview subjects felt that an accident of the Three Mile Island type would be highly improbable in this country, but still possible. About 40' even believed that such an accident could occur in Germany at any time. On the whole, there was nO evidence of a greater confidence in German nuclear power stations. More than three quarters of the interview subjects were convinced that accidents of the Three Mile Island type could also occur in the Federal Republic of Germany. Consequently, a perception· of better safety in German facilities could not have been the cause for the minor shift of opinion after the Harrisburg accident. This lends further weight to the hypotheSiS of a high degree of consolidation of attitudes which cannot be upset by stressing events. - 234 - Nuclear Energy Compared to Other Energy Sources The estimation of nuclear energy as a risk source can best be measured by the evaluation given to this energy source in comparison to alternative sources. The literature con- tains a number of contradictory results on this topic: Maderthaner et al. in 1975 conducted a survey of 148 per- sons in Austria, including persons residing 1n the Vicinity of reactor facilities, where the subjects were asked which of the following technological, industrial or social facilities involved the highest risk rate: a gasworks, a district heating system, an oil refinery, a psychiatric hospital, a nuclear reactor, a prison or an airport. The readiness of the interview subjects to move into the vicinity of one of these facilities or to accept its establishment in their neighborhood was evaluated as an indicator of intllitive risk assessment. Nuclear energy obtained the poorest rating among all seven risk sources. Compared to the other facilities it was perceived and evaluated as the (relatively) moat dangerous and least desirable risk source. Factor analysis revealed two risk types: hazard source due to technological equipment and due to 1magin- ability of hazardous consequences (Maderthaner et al., '76, 236). - Unlike this study, a sampling survey conducted in 1976 in the Federal Republic of Germany revealed precisely the opposite ranking order in the evaluation of risk sources: 54 , of the interview subjects would prefer to move to the vicinity of a nuclear power station, 24 , to the vicinity of a coal-fired power station and 22 , to the vicinity of an oil-fired power station. Other risk sources were not included (Goerke, '76, 131, Part II, p.7). - A survey conducted by the Battelle Institute in different locations in the Federal Republic of Germany where a power station was in eXistence, under construction or 1n the planning phase, revealed a still more differentiated - 235 - pattern. At the outset of this study in 1975 the pre- vailing opinion was that, in the future, nuclear power stations should receive support with preference over all other energy sources. Throughout 1976 the vote in favour of nuclear energy declined significantly. Com- pared to other energy sources such as 011, coal, gas and solar energy, an especially high risk was attributed to nuclear energy. Oil and nuclear energy were given approximately equal evaluations only in one city (Mannheim). In all other locations, nuclear energy always ranked last (Battelle, '76, 25, p. A74ff) . - In a survey conducted by this author in 1977, nuclear energy held the next-to-last rank when once again investigatLng neighborhood preferences. Popularity ranking: machinery-making plant (27 '), motorway (21 '), coal-fired power station (20 t), nuclear power station (18 ') and chemical plant (11 .). In the same study, nuclear energy was evaluated as being especially important for the future, compared to the other energy sources, i.e. 011, gas, solar energy and coal, but at the same time it was associated with an extremely high dislike (Renn, '77, 331, p.56ff). - In another survey conducted in 1978, a coal-fired power station was also more popular as a nelghboarthan a nuclear power station (42 t in favour of coal, 34 • in favour of nuclear energy). However, this survey also revealed that residents with long years of habituation to nuclear energy facilities preferred them on average, while the majority of citizens living in towns with coal-fired power stations would prefer to have nuclear energy facilities in their home towns (Goerke, '78, 132, p.93f). - 236 - The present study confirmed the trend toward an increasingly negative perception of nuclear energy, compared to its alternatives. Coal- and oil-fired power stations were preferred by most interview subjects. Also, nuclear energy exhibited a high polarizing effect: one out of three subjectsselected it first, and about 40 'last. A similar effect can be shown for oil-fired power stations. However, the question as to the undesirable neighborhood of energy-producing systems provides very little information on general acceptance. This variable correlates with the aggregate belief scale on nuclear energy only with a factor of 0.17 (sig. ~ 0.02) and with the general nuclear energy evaluation variable only with a factor of 0.21 (sig. = 0.00). And with the potential voting behaviour in a nuclear energy referendum this variable has a correlation of only 0.11 (sig. - 0.04). Consequently, a preference for coal and oil does not yet mean that nuclear energy is rejected. Rather, this preference merely means than most interview subjects when given a choice as to whether they would prefer coal-fired, oil-fired or nuclear power stations within the limits of their community, gave nuclear energy the lowest rating. Better than from the mere evaluation of energy sources, preferences for energy systems can be determined from the energy option selected by the interview subjects from several given possibilities for a longer forecasting period (in the present case, the year 2000). The interview subjects were requested to state a personal preference sequence of four different energy strategies for the future of energy supply. This resulted in the following values: - 237 - Table 21: Priorities of the Desired Options (in percent) Options First Second Third Fourth Solar energy / alternative 35 28 23 14 energy sources Coal energy 19 29 28 23 Energy conservation 21 27 27 26 Nuclear energy 25 16 22 37 Compared to the three other energy options for the future, nuclear energy ranks last, as it did in the question of neighborhood preferences. The advocates of nuclear energy predominantly assign it the first or second place while almost all the opponents place it last . The correlation coefficient between voting preference in a nuclear energy referendum and the priority vote for pos- sible energy options is 0.67 (gamma) . The correlation with the belief scale (eta ~ 0.62) and with the evaluation of nuclear energy on the basis of a semantic differential is on the sarne level. This relation is illustrated in Fig . 25 .• This close correlation between attitude-forming parameters such as belief system and general evaluation of nuclear energy with the desired future energy supply strategy could have been expected . But it is a surprising fact that energy system preferences undergo a major shift when the interview subjects are not questioned as to their personal preferences but as to the real develop- ments. Table 22 clearly indicates this shift in favour of nuclear energy_ This inversion of Qrder between personal preferences and op-tions considered realistic indicates a gulf between the beliefs - 238 - held by individuals as to what their future should be like, and their expectation as to what it will actually be like. MOre than half the interview subjects were convinced that nuclear energy will have become the most important energy source by the year 2000, but only 19.5 , would welcome this development. This contrast between preference and perceived reality is demonstrated in Fig. 26. In keeping with this estimate of real developments there is also a majority belief that most of the electric power will be generated from nuclear energy by the year 2000. A total of 65 , of the interview subjects attribute first place in this energy sector to nuclear energy, 15% believe in the priority of coal, and only 12 , believe that solar energy and other alternative energy sources will hold first place. At the same time, most of the interview subjects were quite aware that only a very small portion of our present-day electric power is generated from nuclear energy so that, obviously, nuclear energy must be expanded in order to assume its predicted dominant position in the future energy supply. Table 22: Priorities of the Expected Options (in percent) Options First Second Third Fourth Nuclear energy 52 20 18 9 Coal energy 32 39 19 10 Energy conservation 12 25 35 28 Solar energy I alternative 5 16 28 53 energy sources The results obtained in the present study clearly show that perceived future prospects and individual preferences as to the shape of the future are certainly not identical magnitudes but may even be diametrically opposed. This in percent Belief-Scale Semantic Differential Nuclear Energy Last Place Third Place Second Place First Place Rank Order of Nuclear Energy Compared to the Preference for Coal, Solar Energy and Energy Saving - 243 - contradiction is not without relevance for the political acceptance of nuclear energy. The greater the conviction • on the part of the populace that their future way of life is beinq shaped aqainst their will and without any possibility of exertinq personal influence, the sooner we can expect political apathy and resiqnation as well as a retreat into sub-cultures or violent revolt. Anyone who would believe that the climax of the nuclear energy conflict has been passed already (R6thlein, '79, 339) misjudqes the explosive force of a development where the qulf between desired future livinq conditions and perceived reality widens continuously. Another questicnwhich concerned the allocation for research qrants for energy systems in a fictitious budqet of the Federal Ministry of Research and Technoloqy must also be seen under this aspect. The interview subjects were requested to assume the Research Minister's role and allocate the funds for the support of different enerqy options in accordance with their own preferences. Table 23&: Percentaqe Allocation in a Fictitious Enerqy Research Budget Energy Percentage allocations research grants 0 1-10 11-20 21-30 31-50 51-70 Nuclear energy 13 11 9 31 16 8 Fusion energy 38 15 32 9 1 4 Solar energy 0 16 39 34 5 1 Wind ener- gy etc. 4 7 51 36 2 0 Energy conserv. 11 22 46 14 2 2 Coal liqu. and gasi- fication 0 6 27 54 10 3 71-100 12 1 5 0 3 0 - 244 - Table 23b: Mean Values of a Fictitious Energy Research Budget (in percent of the whole budget) Enerqy research All Advocates Opponents grants respondents Nuclear energy 27.4 30.0 21.4 Fusion enerqy 15.8 15.3 13.3 Solar energy 20.1 17.8 25.0 Wind enerqy etc. 20.9 17.3 23.5 Energy conservation 18.5 17 .2 21.3 Coal liqu. and gasification 21.4 20. 2 22.0 - 245 - The response pattern in this imaginary budget planning of research projects lies in the middle between the personal preference for a future energy option and the development considered to be realistic. The interview sub- jects had to abstract themselves from their own person and perform the public function of a federal minister. The phrasing of these questions, however, emphasized the minister's policy-making authority to allocate the budget according to his own judgement. It is not surprising, therefore, that the values range between the extremes of personal preference and forecast future. With the exception of fusion energy, which meant little to the interview subjects, the percentages of the remaining energy sources range in close proximity. The values for coal liquefaction and gasification, solar energy and wind energy do not differ significantly. This applies especially to nuclear energy opponents who allocated 20 , of the research budget to each variant, with the exception of solar energy. The pOisoned water experiment had already revealed that personal risk-taking decisions are influenced by a subject's role as a public servant. And so it is possible that nuclear energy opponents, forced into the research minister's role, abandon personal preferences 1n favour of an overall approach and allocate support to nuclear energy, for instance I for economic reasons. However, the gulf which is revealed between personal preference and perceived development is more likely to be the result of a compensatory attitude in that nuclear energy opponents feel that it is already too late to do anything aqainst the nuclear programme. So they WQul§ allocate financial support to nuclear energy in order to optimize through research the safety aspects of a development which can no longer be stopped. It is likely that, in this case, the two explanation patterns overlap. - 246 - In addition, subdivision of the percentage values into clasles reveals an interesting pattern of association with ex- trl!me values (cf. Table 23a). Apart fran fusion energy which w •• included only because many well-informed interview subjects in the preteats had criticised ita absence, nuclear energy exhibited a greater fringe distribution than all other energy variant.. The high degree of polarization in the nuclear energy debate has induced a number of persons to asaume extreme positions which exhibit almost identical numbera at either end of the opinion scale. However, total rejection of nuclear energy does not imply total advocacy of aolar or wind energyJ nor does extreme adherence to nuclear energy imply total rejection of alternative energy sources. Coal liquefaction and gaSi- fication and energy conservation do not even differ significantly in the contemplated research budgets Buggested by opponents and adherenta of nuclear energy (sig • • 0.34 and 0.11, respectively). The different distribution value. for nuclear energy in comparison to the remaining variants of energy production can be Itill further differentiated by including the lemantic differential for the different alternative. in the analYlis. Such a differential has been plotted in Pig. 27 for coal, nuclear energy and solar energy. The three curves demonstrate that .olar energy i. given highly positive evaluations in all categoriea and that coal is also given a positive evaluation under general and ana- logous terms, with negative judgements only under the pollution and resourc t e was e aapects. However, nuclear energy i. regarded POsitively only in the categories of scientific and modern hil ,w e it i. negatively perceived in all other categories. - 250 - _ A- poa1tive bias scale compria1ng 3 items of the knOWledge leale: _ A negative bias scale compria1ng 3 items of the knOWledge leale: - Interest in the nuclear energy topiCS. In the free alsociations on nuclear energy, negative terms such a8 accidents, radiation risk, harmful effects on health etc. were dominant for both opponents and adherents. The number of these negative judgements is relatively independent of the attitude toward nuclear anergy or its risk estimation. The low correlation coefficient for both variables indicates that, initially, all the interview subjects associated nuclear energy with all the risks involved in it, regardless of whether these risks are considered to be very high or very low (gamma· 0.17). A detailed list of these free associa- tions has been compiled in Table 24. Thla was an open-end question, and the interviewers had to clalsify the responses in accordance with a prescribed list of 18 categories or, when the relponse did not appear to fit .any of these categories, report the response seperately. With respect to the estimation of the interview subjects' own level of information, more than two thirds of the subjects were convinced \hat they were fairly well or wellinfor:med on nuclear energy. In a 1'78 poll, an average of only 40 , of the population stated they were adequately well informed (Goerke, '78, 132, p.122). However, the "adequate" category was the highest possible self-evaluation in that poll. In the survey implemented by this author in 1977, the number of peraona who felt fairly well or well infOrmed was alao much lower (Renn, '77, 331, p.66). However, the two surveys agree in one reapect. most individuals are sceptical with respect to the infor- mation level of other citizens. ~n the 1977 survey 77.7 , and in the present 1979 survey 82 , of the i~terview subjects believed that moat other citizens were poorly - 251 - Table 24. Free Association. on Nuclear Energy (up to five entries per interview subject) Associations Absolute Percentage frequency Percentage of all case Accidents/incidents 229 10.5 45.8 Three 1'l11e Island accident 227 Reproces.ing plants (Gorleben) 173 Citizens' initiatives 129 Economical energy production 127 Modern energy production 126 Radioactivity damage 124 Demonstrations 112 Unsafeneas 91 Reactor types 96 Low risk 61 Environmental degradation 59 Dependability of supply 58 Environmental pollution 56 Efficient energy production 54 Harmful effects on health 51 Environmentally clean 35 War/terrori... 31 Miscellaneous 10 10.5 7.9 5.9 5.8 5.8 5.7 5. 1 4 . 4 4.4 2.8 2.7 2.7 2.6 2.5 2.3 1.6 1.4 0.5 43.4 34.6 25.8 25.4 25.2 24.8 22.4 21.8 19 . 2 12.2 11.8 11.6 11.2 10.8 10.2 7 . 0 6.2 2.0 • zsz • 1 informed on nuclear energy topics. or -fairly poor Y ubj t • self-evaluation of their information While the s ec s id a of the cognitive self-confidence level provides an e of the subjects, the next item of interest is a com- If-evaluation and the objective scale parison between Be th kn ledne iroex. Some numer ical data shall value on e ow ~ illustrate this: Table 25. Variable Xnow- ledge ~ow~and Self-Evaluation of Individuals' Information Level (correlations) Se1£- Evaluation Positive Negattve evaluation of informa- bias bias of informa- tion level tion level of other persona Sign1fi- cance 0.00 0.00 0 . 07 0.02 Correla- tion 0.23 - 0.17 - 0.07 - 0.14 (eta) It is seen clearly from this table that the subjective self-evaluation matches the objective know~ level to a ceresin degree. However. the low correlation coefficient of 0.23 appears to indicate that many individuals o~er­ estimate or underestimate their information level. The better the interview subjects can give correct answers on nuclear energy, the more sceptical they are with respect to the Information level of other citizens. This relation is also fairly weak (eta - - 0.17) but significant. A8 expected, positive and negative bias exhibit a negative correlation with knowledge but the relatively low correla- tion coeffiCient indicates that individual. with high negative or positive bias reject the whole of the opposite bias and probably give the correct answers to pure know~ questions on nuclear energy. High bias, therefore, - 253 - does not necessarily imply les8 information on nuclear energy, but does imply unilaterally weighted knowledge. An interesting difference compared to the 1977 survey in Kerpen de&erves to be mentioned: in that town the nuclear energy advocates clearly exhibited more positive bias than the opponents exhibited negative bias. This relation- ship has become approximately inverted in the present study. The median for positive bias is 1.75 in the case of the adherents, and the median for negative bias on the part of the opponents is 2.21. This relationship applies to all the towns surveyed. In addition to the positive and negative associations and the knowledge scale with the bias indices derived from it, a scale of 13 statements on nuclear energy was used to achieve a broad differentiation of nuclear energy beliefs. The numerical response categories were simply combined in a summated index. This scale is interpreted as a belief scale. Moreover, another, weighted belief scale was de- signed by means of unrotated factor analysis. These two indices have a correlation coefficient of 0.90 so that, as a rule, they are interchangeable. The belief scale exhibits relatively high correlation with all cognitive variables so that, in the further analysis, it was used as being representative for the cognitive component. The emotional intensity within the attitude, used for this nuclear energy survey, was not measured by weighting the statements in accordance with Fishbein's model of the evaluation 8cale, as in the ri8k perception study, but by u8ing a semantiC differential. Based on unrotated factor analysi8, the value. were selected which ranked high in the general evaluation via the 8emantic differential, two other variables - 254 - were computed which are expected to prOvide information on the strength and consistency of the overall con- viction. The strength index was formed from the sum of extreme response categories for discr~natively unique attitude variables (auch as belief, nuclear energy as the most-favoured option etc.). The consistency index describes the linearity of the response behaviour. The numerical value provides information as to the degree to which discr~natory questions yielded responses which matched the positions and attitudes. Table 26. Medians for the Consistency Index and Strength Index Arithmetic medians (Range 1 - 16) Variables Consistency Strength All 8.51 5.34 Adherents 8.10 5.38 Opponents 10.01 6.09 Undecided 6.30 4.13 Kerpen 8.38 4.72 JUl1ch 9.15 5.01 Beverungen 8.04 5.06 Hamm 8.29 5.73 Kalkar 8.76 5.79 It is obvious that nuclear energy opponents and the JUlich and Kerpen interview subjects were more conaistent Ln their response behaviour, although most of the adherents lived in theae two towns Al • ao, Opponents exhibit less compro- mising attitudes i n many nuclear energy topics, while the adherents and und id d ec e subjects prefer moderate res- ponse categories. A high intereat topics and an adequate informa~ion in nuclear energy level have a positive • 255 • effect on the variables of consistency and strength. This tends to confirm the initial hypothesis that the cognitive stress in the case of the adherents, in this case initiated by non-consistent response behaviour and lesser attitude strength, is considerably higher than it is in the case of the opponents who, on the whole, exhibit a more homogeneous response structure. However, the most interesting result drawn from the analysis of energy options is that the opponents reveal a clear discrepancy between their own attitude and their perceived reality. This discrepancy was not noted in the case of the adherents. Thus, individuals who have. developed fairly positive attitudes toward nuclear energy and take the corresponding positions, are often contra- dictory in their cognitive structure, meaning that their belief system may also include information which contra- dicts their overall attitude. But at the same time, they feel that the general development of society confirms their decision. Conversely, nuclear energy opponents consider the development toward expansion of this energy source to be an unavoidable process, but have a consistent and well-developed negative (cognitive and affective) attitude structure. This suggests the conclusion that a positive attitude toward nuclear energy is usually associated with a cognitive internal stress, and a negative attitude with an external realization stress. The last attitude structure component to be discussed isthe behavioural intention. It was already mentioned in the theoretical concept that this propensity cannot be derived from the cognitive and affective components but is only partly attributable to these two factors. In the present study,behaviouraliRtention was measured by means of a scale which combines different statements on possible actions in the nuclear energy conflict. These state- ment. were listed in a sequence so as to obtain an _ 256 - tarting with the collection of excalation of actions, s d din" with active violence. From the signatures an en , 1 index was computed which was label-values on this sea e an 1 i t tion The median values for this led behavioura n en • variable have been compiled in Table 27, below. Table 27. Median Values for the Behavioural Intention Variable, Subdivided by Towns Surveyed and Nuclear Energy Attitudes Arithmetic ~1J8 All Kerpen J(jl1ch Beverungen median Behavioural intention 0 - 30 9.24 10.86 5.01 8.06 Median H ..... Kalkar Adherents Opponents Unde- values cided Behavioural 1ntMltlOn 13.69 11.46 6.6 12.84 4.77 Correct interpretation of the.e data requires a brief ex- planation of the scale intervals. at a range frOID 0 to 30, the value zero means no commitment at all, the value 5 means readinels to sign. declaration or, possibly, go to a meeting, the value 15 includes participation in a demon- stration, a scale value of 25 meanl that the interview subject was in favour of probable participation in a site occupation or removal frem a aite by the police, and the maximum of 30 meanl that the 8ubj ect was in favour of active Violence. The median value of 9.24 for the total number of interview aubjects -meanl that the majority was prepared to o1gn a petition pro or contra nu::lear energy, to go to meetingl oecaaionally and, posaibly, to write letter8 to the editors of newapapers or periodicala. In Hamm and Kerpen the interview aubjects were prepared to take even stronger action, while in J(jlich most citizens - 257 - were reluctant to translate their attitudes into concrete action. However, if the basic nuclear energy attitude is held constant, a relatively uniform distribution of behavioural intention is found in all five survey areas. The differences in behavioural intention are revealed by the scalar medians when the positions are differentiated. The average nuclear energy adherent would sign a petition for a pro-nuclear energy campaign and would perhaps go to a meeting, but he refuses to go any further. The average opponent, however, would go beyond and would write letters to editors, and some are prepared to work for citizens' initiatives and possibly even to take part in demonstrations. The undecided individuals in a referendum could at best be induced to sign a petition pro or CQ'lt ra nuclear energy. This discrepancy between adherents and opponents is atill more evident when the risk-benefit estimate is included as a control parameter. The more extreme the positive or negative perception of nuclear energy as a risk source, the greater will be the propensity to take action. However, the strength of this relation i. only half a8 great for a positive risk estimation a. it is for a negative risk estimation. Fig. l8 shows this relation in graphical form. At first glance, this confusing variety of cognitive, affective and action-relevant parameters fails to reveal any consistent pattern of relationships. However, path analysis, a statistical procedure which allows causal interpretations on the basis of the distribution of correlation coefficients, can be used to explain the basic structure of nuclear energy attitudes. The results of this path analysis have been plotted in Fig. 29 where the numerical data entered above the arrows describe the strength of each relationship. - 258 - The model is characterized by three starting points: the perception of the economic necessity of nuclear energy, knowledge of nuclear energy and the risk-benif1t estimate. These three variables are not independent of each other, but their influence takes different paths. While the belief in the economic necessity of nuclear energy shows very high correlation with the pOlition stated for a hypothetical referendum, the risk-benefit est1mate influences both attitude components, the cogn1t1ve belief system and the affect1ve evaluat10n. Nuclear energy knowledge has only a m1nor direct relat10nsh1p with all of these var1ables. The informat10n level affects the evaluation aud the belief scale only via the circuitous route of self-evaluat10n and posit1ve bias. The strength of the weighted att1tude is obtained from the affective component, the stated bias and the values on the belief scale as w"ll as, indirectly, from the consistency of the verbal responlel in the questionnaire. The consistency in turn is influenced by the evaluation level. The final result,behav- ioural intention, is mostly due to the strength of the stated attitude and, to a le.ser degree, to the position in a referendum on nuclear energy_ With respect to its content, thi8 model can be interpreted as follows: individuals who believe in the necessity of nuclear energy for economic reasons will vote in favour of the nuclear energy source in a referendum, 8~ulated in this study, even when they perceive a greater risk. And their preference for nuclear energy i. amplified if they are convinced of having a good information level and, in addition, have a number of po.itive biaBes for nuclear energy. The belief system, on the other hand, has a greater de- pendence on the perception of the risk-benefit relation. It is true that a po iti s ve risk estimation i. a pre- requisite for a positive attitude in a referendum, • • II c. 01 ... C .E _O = ~ . . :; 0 u ~ . . c c o ;; CO E ~ w :!:: - ., c ., m .;. .!! a: Percepllon of Economic Neec •• - ally of Nucle.r Energy Knowledge about Nuc· I •• r Energy Rlak-Beneflt eatlmatlon Self-Perception o. Own Knowledge pOIIUon In a Referendum on Nucl •• r Energy Positi.e Preduficea BeUef- Scale Evaluation of Nuclear Energy Strength of Attitude Conalstency of Attitude Willingn ••• to Acl (Conation) • 263 - however, it is possible to compensate neqative values on this scale by takinq the bitter pill of economic necessity, althouqh this will not affect the qeneral beliefs and and evaluations of nuclear enerqy. When the risk and the economic necessity are both qiven a neqative evaluation, the result is total rejection which leads to an especially stronq neqative attitude. At the same time, neqative risk-benefit estimations and neqative belief systems require a hiqh internal consistency of the attitude-forminq variables, where coqnitive stress cannot even develop. The strenqth of an attitude, combined with the stated selection preference (pro or contra nuclear enerqyl, can predict a larqe portion of the variance of the behavioural intention variable. MOreover, polls conducted by other institutions suqqest the conolusion that the attitude component. aoquired via risk-benefit estimations have a relatively lonq life, althouqh events suoh as the Three Mile Island aooident do have a destabilizinq effect, but without oausinq any basic ohanqes. The stated basio attitudes or the simulation of a referendum are dependant on the politioal events of the day, sinoe the perceived degree of eoonomic neoessity varies, dependinq on the world-wide politioal situation. The relatively high risk estimation of nuolear enerqy and the preference for alternative energy sources, even on the part of nuclear energy adherent., make it clear that the position oallinq for an expansion of nuolear enerqy as a future option oan be maintained only if there is no doubt that the eoonomio necessity persist. in the peroeption field. It is likely that in times of eoonomic orisis many individuals will vote in favour of nuclear enerqy althouqh, based on their feelinqs and • 264 • t t they would be more likely to have cognitive s rue ure, a sceptical attitude toward this technology. However, theoanverse conclusion does not apply. Committed opponents of nuclear energy who not only perceive the risks of nuclear energy but also dispute its economic necessity will hardly become adherents of this technology even in times of crisis because they have a self-contained, consistent attitude system. 5. Significance of Personal Value Preferences and Socia-Political Beliefs for the Formation of Nuclear Energy Attitudes In recent years an increasing shift from materialistic to post-materialistic, from quantitive to more qualitative values has been noted by social research (Inglehart, 'SO, lS3, p. 144ff, Kmieciak, '76, 20S). Although the polarity between materialist and post-materialistic attitudes is not adequate to describe the value changes which have taken place during the peat decade, the trend shows that, following quantitative aaturation phenomena in the consumer area, objectives aiming at more quality of life are gaining significance. In this conjunction the thesis i. often presented that the rejection of nuclear energy is a value-shift symbol and an expression of diasatisfaction with the consumer culture (Paschen, '79, 307). And, indeed, an earlier study carried out in 1975 revealed a high correlation between the desire for restrictions to economic growth and a negative nuclear energy attitude (van Buiren, '75, 46, p.250). Subaequent .tudies, however, yielded a more differentiated reault (Battelle II, '77, 25, p.l07ff, Renn, '77, 331, p. 90ff). the majority of the population attempt to take a middle course between materialiatic and poat-materialiatic valuea without allocating any clearly defined priorities. 265 -- In the present study. 17 general problems of society such as unemployment. terrorism or health care. and 10 per- sonal values such &8 a clean environment or harmonious family life were specified in a scale. These terms had not been selected arbitrarily but determined through open-end questions in a pretest. In the actual interviews the subjects were requested to state the urgency and importance of these problems and values. The responses mostly confirmed the results of more recent studies in this field that there is a high degree of competition between materialistic and post-materialistic values in the eyes of the population and that priorities are established in a differentiated and ambivalent manner. Abstract problems of SOCiety such as unemployment, terrorism and inflation are classified as being seriOllS while, At the same time, personal values such as health. clean environment and harmonious family life are estimated highly. Purely material values such as raising the standard of living or high income are more likely to be found at the bottom end of both 8cales. Environmentalism. an important post-materialistic value. is given the greatest urgency with respect to both society and the personal environment. Other typical categories in this area such as social justice and enjoy- ment of one's work. occupy intermediate positions. Comprising factor analysis allowed plotting of the SOCiety-related and personal value beliefs in a two-factor diagram whose axes represent the degree of post-materialistic qualitative value commitment and the degree of personal involvement (cf. Fig. 30). However. the structure of value beliefs is of little interest for the question as to nuclear energy attitudes. More significant is the relationship existing between nuclear energy attitude and value system which shall be illustrated with the aid of some correlation analyses. - 269 - Both tables demonstrate that the stated value beliefs with the exception of the clean environment variable have only a minor effect on nuclear energy positions and attitude.. Again, it is interesting to note the greater tie. between economdc concepts and voting behaviour in a referendum (position), and between en- vironment- or risk-specific values and the attitude components. That the energy crisis and nuclear energy have a discriminating effect on both positions is not surprising, however, the correlation coefficient is unexpectedly low. Nuclear energy adherents prefer economLcally quantitative values, and nuclear energy opponents prefer post-materialistic and qualitative values, but this relationship is weak with the excep- tion of quite obvious relations (with respect to nuclear energy and the energy crisis). The only exception is that a spectal preference for a clean environment haa a great effect on the belief system on nuclear energy. Negative perceptions of nuclear energy are especially well-developed in those individuals who attribute special value to a clean environment. An interesting feature is that the general attitude toward the environ- ment protection problem provides a much poorer predic- tive value for the nuclear energy attitude than the individualized desire for a clean environment. Quite generally, environmental protection 1. an ~portant society-related problem for the adherents of nuclear energy a8 well . In addition to differentiation between opponents and adherents, the category of the undecided individuals must be discussed in more detail here, since it is this group in particular which takes a pro-materialistic and pro-economic position in the survey, which would have been more in keeping with the attitude of a nuclear - 270 - energy adherent. The attitude of the undecided indivi- duals becomes clearer if we look once again at the risk-benefit evaluation (medians. adherents +1.7, opponents -0.9 and undecided individuals -0.5). Due to their negative risk perception they cannot state a positive attitude toward nuclear energy, although this would match their economic value beliefs. Yet, these economic beliefs prevent a complete shift to the opposite side. Of cour.e, thi. dualism of nega- tive risk estimation and po.itive economic evaluation does not apply to all undecided individuals I however, the medians in each case indicate that a majority of this group i. involved in an inner conflict between risk evaluation and economic value-orientation. This thesis is further confirmed by the fact that, in the question A8 to the economic nece •• ity of nuclear energy, the undecided individuals are much closer to the position of the adherents than to the poa1tion of the opponents (medians. adherents 2.1, undecided individuals 1.7 and opponents 0.2). In a number of studies the clos. proximity of socio- political beliefs and nuclear energy estimations has been determined in addition to the value commitment. In order to study thi. hypothesis in more detail, the follOWing attitudes described in theoretical or empiri- cal studies reported in the literature were meaaured with the aid of suitable scale •• - a confidence scale for science, politics and technology (models. Douglin, '76, 811 Goerke '78, 132)1 - a conservatism scale (model. Dumanil, '77, 85)1 - a ' participation scale (model. Douglin, '76, 81)1 • 272 • The results of this reqression analysis have been compiled in Table 31, below. Table 311 Multiple Reqreasion Analysi. of Socio-Political Attitudes with the Belief System (Belief Scale) R R2 r Independent Multiple Declared Simple Siqn1fi- variables correlation variance correlation canCe 1- COnfidence scale 0.49 0.24 0.49 2. Environmentalism 0 •. 56 0.31 0.35 scale 3 . Participation scale 0.60 0 . 36 0.41 4. COnservatilm Beale 0.62 0.38 0.29 5. Political apathy 0.63 0.39 0.41 acale Conaequently, almost 40' of the belief aystem'. variance can be explained by the combined effects of these five scales. Of special siqnificance are confidence in science and technoloqy and the atrenqth of environ- mental awareness . In addition, the desire for partici- pation has an amplifyinq effect on a neqative nuclear enerqy evaluation • . The magnitude of the correlation values quite supporta the assumptiona that qeneral aocio-political attitudes 0.00 0.00 0.00 0 .03 0.02 are aiqn1ficant for nuclear enerqy perception. The.e findinqa are alao lupported by the hiqh correlation values with the riak-benefit estimatea. All five relationshipa to this variable have been plotted in Fiqure 3' . Consequently, low confidence in the statements of scien- tists and technoloqiats, combined with a hiqh priority for environmental protection mean a more neqative nuclear enerqy risk perception from the outset. flO, Confidence In Science and Technology Anti-Fatalistic Point of View Tendency toward Con.ervatlve Social Velue. Environmental Awarene •• Desire for Participation Risk-Benefit Estimation - 275 - COnversely, confidence in Bcience and technology and a low degree of environmental awareness represent a startinq attitude which tends to develop positive expected values for the nuclear energy attitude. In spite of theae convincinq results, over 60' of non-explained variance remains. This demonstrates that the deterministic relationship between the socio- political attitude field and nuclear energy attitudes, postulated by Dumenil et aI, can hardly match real conditions, althouqh it was impossible to verify all possible, related attitudes within the scope of the present study. Effect of Social and Demoqraphic Characteristics on Nuclear Energy Attitudes Relationships which may be revealed in studies of the interrelationship of attitudes and external criteria such as sex, age, class membership etc. do not, at this stage, provide any information on their cauEal structure. Rather, correlations are indicative of related patterns of the response behaviour which, depending on the significance of the relationship, can no longer be dismissed as randall phenomena. This preliminary remark is especially important in the case of the 80clal structural characteristics because, as a rule, they can be interpreted neither as causative factors nor as consequences. In most eases, third factors can be dete~lned (intervening variables) which have an effect on both the social characteristics and the phenomena requiring explanation. Thus, sex in itself is hardly ever the orlqinal cause of any behaviour, but the role aSSOCiated with sex, specific types of education or training, or even stereotype self-perception. Therefore, althouqh the following discussion describe. some cross-relations between nuclear energy attitudes and social characteristics - 277 - Sex-specific differences are also found in correlation analysis with attitude-forming variables. Women evaluate the nuclear energy risk higher than men and, consequently, also have more negative beliefs. Their attitude structure is less well developed and their behavioural intention slightly lower. This may be due to the fact that they occupy neutral positions more frequently. An interesting feature is the relationship to the "economic necessity of nuclear energy"variable where there is no difference between men and women and where the correlation coefficient is even slightly negative (- 0.07, not sig- nificant) • At the same time, women are especially open- minded with respect to alternative energy sources as desirable options for the future. OVer 45 • of the female nuclear energy adherents opted for solar energy in the first place, ahead of nuclear energy. The discussion of these results is highly speculative. It is likely that women react with much greater sensitivity to risks which have detrimental effects on health, and are guided more by archetypal beliefs such as the Sun as a life-giving power (WUnschmann, '80, 460). However, the value beliefs and the socio-political attitudes included in the questionnaire revealed almost no significant relationships which would further support this interpretation. While it is true that a large proportion of women are convinced of the economic necessity of nuclear energy, this Aspect apparently bas a lesser effect on the evaluation of the different beliefs. Consequently, risks to human 11fe and to the environment have priority over economic objectives. Perhaps men are more receptive to economic necessities due to their direct involvement in the work environment and can therefore compensate risks which they perceive as negative. Women, on the other hand! are more remote from business and, while perceiving economic developments and constraints, subordinate them to their personal environment. • 278 • By~!c!£_~n!t9X-~ttl~~Cl-!D~_~9! The relationship between age and nuclear energy attitudes i8 lel8 clearly defined than the relationship to 8ex . A linear trend cannot be identified. A more negative attitude 1s found in the 25-36 age group, a positive high in the 18-25 age group, average values in the middle age groups and, again, a 8lightly negative attitude in the group ovsr 56. These data largely match the re8ults of the 1977 8tudy where a preponderance of negative attitudes was found in the 26-35 age group (Renn, '77, 331, p. 109). It i8 pollible that thil distribution is caused by 8uperimposing conventional and modern innovation protest, a phenomenon already diacus8ed in the theoretical part. Traditional individuala of advanced age who are frequently opposed to innovation in their living environ- ment tend to exhibit the same rejection of the use of nuclear energy as the younger generation atill affect&d by the unre8t of the sixtiea, while the very young prefer a positive nuclear energy attitude because they believe in progre88 and the 35 to 40 age group prefer power stattons for reasons of aocial advancement. IIl1clelll:_tuel:a¥_litt1tIlI1CI_IIII1_!1!;rU!!!!:!H2!! The cla88 membership characteristic comprioe8 data on income, occupational prestige and educational level. Again, it is of intere8t to determine the degree to which the aocial preatige of an individual can be related to hi8 or her nuclear energy attitude. Starting out from the simple correlation values between etratum and attltude- forming variables, it i8 found that there i8 a marked effect on nuclear energy knowledge and a lesser effect on the b&haMioural intention, general belief syotem and negative bias. Th .. connection between knOWledge and cla.. is not ouprising, since it io· expected that individuals of higher education level and occupational · prestige would e180 have - 279 - a higher information level. Of greater theoretical interest is the fact that negative nuclear energy attitudes and propensity to take actions increase slightly with higher class membership. This demonstrates that the positive correla- tion between class membership and nuclear ener~ attitude which was found years ago can no longer be confirmed. This relationship becomes even more evident when the class variable is subdivided into its basic quantities. Income still exhibits a positive correlation with the nuclear energy attitude (r - 0.17), while occupational prestige and education have a negative effect (- 0.19 and - 0.24, respectively) . All of these values are Sig- nificant on the 99 , confidence level. Apparently, there- fore, an inversion in the soclal make-up of nuclear energy opponents and adherents has taken place in recent years, an effect also reported in the United States and Canada. This shift demonstrates an inversion of the innovation protest from a conservative, more ignorant and soc1a1ly defensive rejection attitude to a progressive, educationally elitist and socially offensive movement. Initially, this change is taking place on a cognitive basis only, while the affective classification lags behind, since the general evaluation failed to reveal any significant differences between the members of the different social classes. This pattern of class-specific innovation protest can be drawn with still greater clarity if the correlation values of the stratification index are diffe~entlated by attitudes. These data have been compiled in Table 33, below, and plotted in a graph (cf. Fig. 33). With increasing class level the opponents exhibit an overproportional growth in nuclear energy knowl~ and behavioural intention. Also, they show a greater firmness (strength) of attitude than the adherents who tend to become more sceptical in their views and risk-benefit • 200 • Table 33. Effect of Attitudes Opponenta Stratification on Nuclear Energy (Subdivided into Nuclear Energy and Adherents' Variable Correlation coefficients Pearson r Nuclear energy risk estimation - 0.12 Xnowledge 0.27 - Negative bias =-Q.:.11 Belief system - 0.18 ------ Nuclear energy evaluation - 0.08 Nuclear energy safety - 0.11 coal evaluation - 0.17 ----- Solar energy evaluation + 0.12 Attitude strength 0.06 Attitude consistency ~ Action propensity 0.17 ---- n 422 ------- p - 0.95 significance p - 0.99 significance Pearson r Pearson r adherents opponents - 0.26 - 0.04 0.16 0.34 - 0.03 - 0.16 - 0.14 + 0.13 - 0.02 - 0.11 - 0.14 - 0.07 - 0.15 - 0.18 ------ ------ 0.14 0.11 - 0.05 Q!.l~ 0.18 0.28 - - 0.11 Q"ll 187 144 In percent Knowledge on Nuele.r Queotlono (Oppanenta 01 NucIHr Enernl Knowledge on Nuele.r Queollono (Proponenta 01 Nuclear EMfIIJI Willing .... to Take Aellon (Opponental Wllllngn •• to Toke Aellon (Proponental Strength 01 Attltudln.1 Commitment (Opponent., Strength 01 Attitudinal Commitment (Proponento) Low Lower Stratum High Lower Stratum Low Middle Stratum High Middle Stratum High Stratum • 283 - estimations with higher social position and, accordingly, became less extreme in their attitudes and behavioural intentions. These results obviously show that opponents tend to reinforce their attitudes with higher social position, becoming still more extreme, still more prepared to take action than they would be on the average, while the adherents tend to become more careful in their statements with higher social position, and are only slightly more prepared to take action. At the same time, opponents in higher social positions exhibit better knowledge on nuclear topics. It is likely that, due to the public discussion and due to the stress caused by the incongruity between their own attitude and their perceived reality, opponents in the upper social positions have concerned themselves more intensively with nuclear energy problems, while the adherents from the upper social strata attempt to overcome their internal incongruity in this respect by suppressing the topic. This assumption cannot be fully verified within the scope of the present study, but the values obtained for the two groups suggest such an interpretation. It should be noted in passing that interview subjects of all classes were equally interested in environmentalism as a personal or society objective and exhibited relatively similar values on the environmentalism scale (r - 0.10; eta - 0.14, sig - 0.05). This i. contradictory to the hypothesis that the relative de- privation of the middle classes is the cause for their commitment to environmentalism. Rather, it appears to become evident that behavioural intention and commitment correlate highly with class membership, causing a greater representation of the middle classes in the organized environmentalist groups. This has also been the result of the present study where, unlike the members of the lower claa.e8, more than double the members of the upper cla88 and upper middle cla.s stated that they gave some kind of support to citizens' initiative •• 1I1l1:1IlU_lilllrlU_MtUIIIlt:I_DII,LEalitiI:Dl_En!;1I_Eut.lltlllll:lI One of the salient characteristics of the nuclear conflict is that it form. a front straight across the institutions of society and the political parties. Just recently, the political success of the environmentalists has initiated a new polittcal polarization which, however, cannot over- come the current leftist-rightist dualism of today's politics but at best expand it by yet another dimension. Both the split across the political parties and the opening of a second front can be seen from the data on votIng preferences compiled in Table 34. The voters of the parties represented in the Bundestag (West German Parliament) are clearly differentiated into opponent., adherents and neutral.. The trend that COO (Christian Democratic Party) voters have a more positive attitude toward nuclear energy than the voters of the Government coalition parties, has become stronger. This may be due to the fact that the CDO has taken a clear-cut decision in favour of nuclear energy which has been sub- ject to little dispute within the party, ·and that the affinity of COO voters to business-related values supports the basic tendency toward attributing positive weight to nuclear energy. This is supported by the high correlation between CDU party preference and busine.a- related objectives (lambda - 0.23/ SPD (Social Democratic Party) party preference. lambda - 0.14) and between CDO party preference and the belief in the economic necessity of nuclear energy. On the other hand, the risk-benefit eltimation does not differ Significantly between SPD and COO voters. - 286 - ~he proportion of opponent I is unu8ually high among the FOP (Liberal Democratic Party) voters. However, the possibIlity of a measuring artefact cannot be discounted in spite of the 8tati8tically proven significance of the difference to COO and SPD voter., lince the data base includes only 29 casea. Surprisingly high is the proportion of environmentalist voters. Since thia cateqory included 4' persona, the data base i. somewhat broader and yields more information. The frequent nomination of environmentalist groups a. the preferred party vote is probably due to desires for con- sistency (being opposed ,one mIght as well say one votes for the environmentalista) or to demo.caple distortion (en- vironmentaHsts hardly ever refused to take part in the interviews). Since this study did not concern a voting forecaat but relationshlps between voting preference and attitude-relevant variablea, the high frequency of environ- mentalist voters 18 even welcome from the analytical polnt of view • . The median values of a number of variables for the po- tential voters of an environmentalist party have been compiled in Table 35, below, in order to obtain a better profile of environmentalilt voters. Baled on the value. 1n this table, the environmentalist voter. are clearly opposed to nuclear enerqy and in a number of attitude-forming variables take even more extreme poal tiona than the average for the nuclear energy opponent.. They exhibit a great environmental awarenea. and partiCipation proponlity and have loat most of their confidence in the expertl of acience and technology. They are not better informed than the other interview subjects but bave ·1e.8 bias on nuclear energy than .011· the inaividuala who would vote againot nuclear energy Table 35: Structural data on environmentalist voters (median value comparison) lledJ.an Belief Affective Nuc. enerqy Solar energy Confidence Conserva- Participa- Environmen- values Beale evaluation safety evaluation scale tin Beale tion scale tallam scale n Total average -1.89 3 . 22 1.26 12.7 5 .62 27.9 28;6 16. 9 503 Environmen- taliat votera -49.4 -11.4 -8.4 16.8 -3.5 13 . 1 21:4 30.4 41 T-Te.t Siq. (99)) ye. ye. yeo yea ye. yea yea ye. lCnowJ..edge Negative P08itive Behavioural Strength Consistency Aqe Clas. Sex n bias bias intention of attitude of attitude Sl .... Total averagle 4.7 1.6 1.1 9.2 5.3 8.5 42 2 . 6 1.3 503 Environaen- ta1iat voters 5. I 1.8 0.2 17,8 7. 4 9 . 2 31 3.1 1.4 41 T-Teat no no Yea yeo yea Siq. (99') no yea yeo no • 288 • in a referendum. Their nuclear energy attitude is firm, consistent and action-relevant. They are prepared to take far-reaching action above and beyond the average level stated by all opponents. Finally, the environ- mentalist voters include very many young people and members of upper social strata. These data have been confirmed by analyses of voting be- haviour in the Federal State of Baden-WUrttemberg where younger voter. and persona in the upper social structure more frequently voted for environmentaltst parties than older or socially less privileged persons. The inter- pretation of these data is self-evident: when very negat1ve evaluations of nuclear enerqy exist and, at the same time, confidence in the technical competence of scientists, engineer. and planners has been lost, and when, moreover, there 18 a high propensity to translate one's attitude. into actlon, a threshold value 18 exceeded where the previous party preference 18 abandoned in favour of & shift to an environmentalist party. This threshold, of course, 18 lowest where the patterns of voting behaviour have not yet become very fixed, meaning that younger people are probably more inclined to opt for environmentalilt parties than older peraona who have voted for one of the big parties (pos.ibly different parties at different times) over many year. . Since there i. a high correlation between ~aviouralintent.1on and stratifi- cat,ionand, at the same time, behaviouralintentlon is one of the prerequisite. for changing to an environmentalist party, peraon. fram upper 80cial clal.ea are a180 found more frequently among the potential voters wr environmentalist parties. • 289 - ~~~!!!t_§~!~_~~~!~yg!!_!~~-2~b!t_§2£!!!.£!~~2t!!! In addition to the indifference of nuclear energy attitudes toward the political spectrum which, however, has been the subject of greater polarization in the meantime - as shown above - almost all studies on this aubjecthave revealed that social category characteristics are independent of nuclear energy attitudes. Neither religious confession, marital status, occupation, nor household size or other variables of this type have been revealed as discriminatory quantitiea in empirical studies. This lack of influence has been fully confirmed in the present study. only education, income and occupational prestige have an effect on nuclear energy attitudes. These, however, have already been taken into consideration jOintly through the stratification variable. Confidence in Institutions and Reference Groups Man's attitude toward nuclear energy can be based only to a slight extent on his personal experience, rather, information on the nuclear conflict 1s received via the communication media and processed subjectively. Since most individuals are unable to verify the correctness of this, frequently contradictory, information, the credibility image gains a special weight. It is no longer the infor- mation content which i. the yardstick for cognitive judgement, but the perceived social situation involved in the information transfer and the estimation of the information source. Based on the previous results of the analysis of the confidence scale, a marked difference between nuclear energy opponents and adherents would be expected in the attribution of credibility to the institutions of society. ·m· And, indeed, nuclear energy opponents are more sceptical toward moat institutions, but not nearly as much a8 we ~ould have assumed. The numerical data have been compiled in Table 36, below. This table provides information on a number of circumstances. Where credibility in nuclear energy questions is attributed to anyone at all, it is most likely to be the representatives of science and technology or of technically responsible political institutions (such as the Federal Ministry of Research and Technology). With respect to assessment of cred1b1l1ty of sc1entif1c inst1tut10ns there are only minor differences between nuclear energy opponents and adherents. Both groups have the greatest confidence in these institutions. Central political institutions and technically competent business institutions predominantly occupy intermediate positions on the scale, while general institutions of society such aa trade unions and churches, and non- specific social position. such as journalist, manager or local politician, determine the bottom end of the scale. Opponents and adherents hardly differ in the basic, tripartite scale attribution: science first, followed by politics in general, and finally the institutions of SOCiety. On the whole, however, the opponents assigned lower grades to almost all institutions than the adherents. With respect to the credibility of specialized 8cience, opponents and adherents attribute the greatest credibility to universities and nuclear research centres. This is • 291 - Table 36. Attributed credibility of institutions in nuclear enerqy questions (Medians; ~anqe: 1 - hiqh credibility 4 - zero credibility) Institution Total _ian Med1an valu •• value Opponenta Adherent. Profe.sor at a nuclear research centre 1.44 1.88 1.47 Scientist at a university 1.72 1.88 1. 71 Reactor safety coamU8.1on 1.95 2.42 1.80 Federal M1nister of Re- learch and Technology 2.09 2.34 1.99 Scientist in a citizens' action qroup 2.32 2.18 2. SO Representative of the medical profesBion 2.39 2.42 2.38 Federal Chancellor 2.40 2 .68 2.36 Spokesman f or a citizens' action group 2.60 2.39 2. 73 Chairman of the board 2.69 2 . 91 2.43 of a utility company TV commentator 2. 70 2.77 2.67 Spokesman for Social Democratic party (SPO) 2.95 3.06 2.84 Spokesman tor Christian Democratic Party (CDU) 2.96 3.03 2. 86 Politician 2.99 3.07 2.95 Spokesman for Liberal Democratic Party (FOP) 3.00 3.00 3.01 Journalist 3.03 2.97 2. 96 D1rector of an lnCluBtrial company 3 . 10 3.32 2.89 Local politician 3.12 3.17 3.06 Trade union leader 3.20 3. 24 3. 12 Clergyman 3.56 3.37 3.40 Chairman of the German Football Aa8oclation 3. 87 3.84 3.68 Averaqe value 2.71 2.80 2. 64 Confidence interval :!9.23 -+0.64 -+0.38 - - T-Teat 0 . 00 0.03 0 . 00 0.00 0.00 0.60 0 .00 0.00 0.00 0.18 0.01 0 .04 0.09 0.93 0.87 0 .00 0. 15 0.10 0.64 0.01 0 . 01 (p) • 292 • surprising because the opponents are convinced that the overwhelming majority of the members ' of these two institutions are in favour of nuclear energy. The competent scientists of the citizens' initiatives are given only third place by the nuclear energy opponents (and eighth place by the adherents). This would not have been expected, considering the homogeneous belief structure of the opponents. How should these data be interpreted? All the interview subjects agree that statements on nuclear energy by technically competent institutions have the greatest credibility. Political and SOCiety institutions are given lesser credibility ratings with decreasing attributed technical competence. The greater the position-bound attitude of scientific institutions, such as citizens' initiatives or the Reactor Safety Commission, the greater becomes their discriminatory effect on positions and attitudes. An almost identical result was found in Goerke's study where the interview subjects predominantly explained the confidence loss of scientific and business institution. by their dependence on fixed institutional objectives (Goerke, '79, 132, p.123). Non-affiliated institutions such a. the universities are equally accepted by both sides, although even the nuclear energy opponent. believe that moat professors would vote 1n favour of nuclear energy 1n a referendum. The nuclear reaearch centres range between the affiliated and non-affiliated institutions. It appears that the nuclear energy adherents require more backing by their confidence group than the opponents who are relatively "generous" in attributing a high degree of credibility even to institutions holding the opposite view, such a. the Reactor Safety Commission and the nuclear research centres. It is possible that the . - 293 - opponents can rest more dependably on their consonant attitude structure and, based on this inner certainty, can be more receptive to information which contradicts their personal attitude, while the adherents, uncertain in their inner attitude structure, require strong support by institutions. Economic functionaries, especially private business functionaries, are given the lowest credibility potential by the opponents. It appears that they have lost their stock of potential persuasive power. Beyond the credibility question, it is of interest to determine the degree to which potential reference groups, i.e. qroups of peraons whose attitudes and behaviour are emulated by other peraons, have an effect on individuals' nuclear energy attitudes. Direct measurement of reference group influence is hardly possible in a sampling survey. Therefore, an indirect procedure was used: all the inter- view subjects were requested to state how their friends, family members, members of the big political parties, communists, members of citizens' initiatives, university professors and nuclear energy experts would vote 1n a nuclear energy referendum . In addition, they were asked to predict the probable outcome of ~he referendum. The results have been summarized in Table 37, below. This table show.~ointerest1ng counter-trends: although most interview subjects would vote for nuclear energy, the majority believes that nuclear energy would be defeated in such a referendum. Among the nuclear energy adherents, approximately one third would not even dare to make a foreca while another third predicted that their own position would be defeated in a referendum. On the other hand, 50 • of the opponents are convinced that their attitude would win in a referendum. However, even in this qroup one out of four believes that the adherents would win. This first counter-trend has been confirmed - 295 - and family, professors and members of the three biq political parties. The question immediately arises as to why the.e opponents believe that nuclear enerqy would fail in a referendum while, at the same time, they classify their primary and secondary reference qroups as beinq pro nuclear enerqy. In order to study this question in more detail, the predictions qiven for the votinq behaviour of friends and family were differentiated by positions and these values were compared to the subjects' own votinq behaviour. Even after this break-down the paradoxical sitUation remain. that nuclear enerqy opponents are much less certain that their family and friend. share their opinions. Amonq the adherents, 77 • believe that their family, and 62 • that their friends would vote in favour of nuclear enerqy. Amonq the opponents,only half are convinced that the members of their primary reference qroups are also opponents of nuclear anerqy. The undeCided individuals also feel that more adherents than opponents of nuclear enerqy are found in their primary environment. The difference between the prediction for the whole (nuclear enerqy will be defeated in a referendum) and the pre- diction for the social environment (f~ily and friends would vote in favour of nuclear enerqy) is remarkable and difficult to explain. Further subdivision of the reaulta by sex and class membership also failed to reveal any better startinq point fer meaninqful interpretation. It is likely that the frequent reports on nuclear enerqy protests have so solidified public opinion that unknown individuals are predsmtnantly perceived as opponents, while positive attitude. toward nuclear enerqy frequently dominate 1n one's own aocial environment. It 1s a180 possible that, similar to the credibility assessment, the inconsistency of the attitude structure is compensated via an inner dependence on the attitudes of friends and family, thus reducinq coqnitive stress. .. -- - -.- - 296 - The pattern of reference qroup effects becomes even more complex when the secondary reference groups, here repre- sented by the political parties, are included. Since the voting preferences of the interview subjects are available, they can be compared to the presumed voting behaviour of party members. It is found that the nuclear energy opponents also feel that the majority of the members of their preferred po- litical party (with the exception of the environmentalist parties) are clearly pro nuclear energy. In this respect the party for which they vote apparently does not represent their position, and this i. clearly perceived. This inter-party separation line between nuclear energy opponents and adherents is even more clearly revealed if the data on the credibility of party spokesmen are included. Most nuclear energy adherents tend to classify the spokes- man for their pol1 tical party as credible in nuclear energy questions. Thi. applies especially to SPD (Social Demo- cratic Party) voters and to a lesser degree to CDU (Chris- tian Democratic Party) voters, but not to FOP (Liberal Democratic Party) voters. However, this deviation is not significant due to the small data base of FOP voters. Nuclear energy opponents among the CDU voters trust their own party spokesman less than adherents among the CDU voters would trust the spokesman of the competing SPD. This relationship is .imilar in the case of the SPD. If one attempts to retain an overall impression from these data, the idea of • .hift of political fronts practically auggests itself. The spl1 t of the pol1 tical parties in the nuclear energy ·que.tion has led to a solidarity movement across party boundarie •• seek support from adherent. in all Nuclear energy adher.ents other parties and in this question disregard all the differences existing • 297 • between the parties. The opponents, on the other hand, feel that they are not properly represented by any political party and, therefore, withdraw credibility in nuclear energy questions even from their own pre- 5m1ed~. The degree to which this new environmentalist front exists for this particular topic only and does not appear with respect to other problems, cannot be deter- mined from the available data . However, there are indications that this third dimension has gained access to the political perception of the population. Attitude of the Population to Specific Questions of Nuclear Energy Use In addition to questions on attitude structure and its influencing parameters, aome specific problems of nuclear energy use were oonsideredin this sampling survey. These problems, for ' instance the waste disposal problem caused by nuclear power stations, are also relevant for the overall nuclear energy attitude, but cannot be con- sidered dependent or independent variables/ rather, they are supplementary elements in the cognitive image of nuclear energy in the eyes of the population. The evaluation of these interview data yields some interesting information on the apan of the perception field, but beyond that aspect has only minor importance for the overall theoretical frame of reference . Therefore, the results ahall be reported in summary· form only. !~2ngm!~_g£~h_~n~_!n!£~_~2n!~E~!2n Previous analyses had revealed a clear trend toward a positive evaluation of nuclear energy whenever the interview subjects exhibited a more busineaa-related value attitude. This resulted, from the value priorities of opponents and adherents and from the belief in the economic necessity of a further expansion - 298 - h f r it was aatura1 to of nuclear energy. T ere 0 e, include a specific question on the possibility of e11minatinq the ties between economic qrowth and enerqy consumption, in ' addition to the general value priorities and economic aspects of nuclear energy. After all, there had been very considerable discussion in the media in 1979 as to whether economic qrowth processes must, of necessity, be tied to a growth in energy consumption. The answers qiven by the interview subjects have been compiled in Table 38, below. Table 38: Replies in Elimination of Ties Between Economic Growth and Energy Conaumption percent All Kerpen JUlich Beverunqen Hamm Kalkar Ties can be eliminated 51 44 50 59 60 46 Ties cannot be eliminated 44 50 48 35 34 51 No opinion 5 6 2 6 7 3 . Up to a few year. ago, nobody would have doubted a linear relationship between economic qrowth and the qrowth of energy consumption. The discussion initiated by scientific publications on this topic haa found auch a broad response in public opinion within a minimum of time, that over 50 • are convinced today that ,economic qrowth can be achieved without increasing energy C()nIWllpE1..o!l'. ·Of course, the objection to thia will be that the interview subjecta were overtaxed with auch a question so that the result must be considered 118 reaponse set. However this is contra- dicted by the clear-cut phrasinq of the question, by the relatively rare use of the "don't know"response and by the similarity of the diatribution with respect to related variables. The nuclear energy opponent'. position - 299 - is even more vividly revealed by this result. based on the idea that a reasonable living standard and economic progress can be achieved even without energy growth, he can let himself be induced by his negative risk perception to forgo the use of nuclear energy without having to fear serious disadvantages. This is all the more easy for him because the quantitative values of economic growth mean little to him in any event and because he assigns first priority to the protection of the environment. ~~~!~g~!_!~!~~_£!~!!!~!~_!n!~!!~!Y!! Analysis of citizens' initiatives is given great attention in the social sciences literature on protest movements against nuclear energy. Within the scope of the present study this topic was only marginally included in order to compare attitudes toward citizens' initiatives with nuclear energy attitudes. In addition to the repeatedly cited study by the Battelle Institute which dates back to 1977, two more recent 1979 studies are available which, in conjunction with nuclear energy attitude measurements, were also devoted to the attitude of the population toward citizens' initiatives (Instltut fUr Demoskople, Allenabach, '79, and Goerke, '79, 132). According to these two sources, most of the interview subjects believed that. _ Citizens' initiatives acted in the interest of the general public and less in the interest of the persons involved; _ Citizens' initiatives did not aim at achie~ing material advantages for their members; _ Citizens' initiatives were not predominantly influenced by leftist or rightist fractional groups (although this was believed, nonetheless, by about 30 'l. - 300 - These data are based on representative samples, 1n one case covering the entire Federal Republic of Germany (Allensbach), and in the other case several towns (Goerke). On the whole, there is a trend over time toward increasing identification of citizens' initiatives with the public interest. However, the diapersion spread has been relatively high since 1975 (cf. Allensbach, '79). In the present study the interview subjects were presented three statements on citizens' initiatives and requested to select the correct answer. Table 39 provides a review of the statements and percentage values for all survey units. Table '39: Confirmation of Statements on Citizens ' Initiatives (in percent) Statement All Kerpen JUl1ch Beverunqen Hamm Xalkar 1. Cltlzens' ini- tiati ves re- present only 10 14 5 12 3 13 their own interests 2 . Cltlzens' ini- tiati ves are indications 63 61 73 57 70 54 that abuses exist 3.Cit1zens' ini- tiatives should have participa- 27 25 tion ri.ghta 22 31. 27 33 Citizens' initiatives are attributed a broad functional area_especially in those _towns .where ci.t1zens' ini.t1atives contra nuclear energy exist, or have existed in the past, but at the same time they are moat frequently attributed self-seeking motives in these towns. In 'JUlich, Kerpen and Hamm (where .a weaker citizens' initiative exists) the "soft" middle